U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Front Psychol
  • PMC10399695

Cancel culture can be collectively validating for groups experiencing harm

Associated data.

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: https://osf.io/qftn8/ .

Introduction

Social psychological research on collective action and intergroup harm has yet to adequately consider the potential role of cancel culture or feelings of collective validation in motivating collective action. The current research will begin to fill this gap and may broaden our understanding of the psychological mechanisms that inspire and maintain collective action in response to intergroup harm. To our knowledge, this research is the first social psychological analysis of the impact of cancel culture on collective action and as means for producing feelings of collective validation.

In two experimental studies, participants read a story describing an event of discrimination against their group followed by a manipulation of the presence or absence of an episode of cancel culture. Study 1 samples woman university students ( N = 520) and focuses on their responses to a sexist incident on campus. Study 2 (pre-registered) assesses the generality of the model in a racism context with a community sample of East Asian Canadians and Americans ( N = 237).

Study 1 showed that an episode of cancel culture had an indirect positive effect on collective action intentions mediated by feelings of collective validation and collective empowerment. Study 2 showed the indirect effect of cancel culture on collective action intentions mediated by feelings of collective validation and collective anger and contempt.

The current research offers a novel theoretical and empirical introduction to the concept of collective validation and the understudied context of cancel culture to the existing social psychological research and theory on collective action. Further, cancel culture has been criticized as problematic. However, this perspective centres those in positions of power. Through this research, we hope to shift the focus onto marginalized groups’ perspectives of episodes of cancel culture. This research shows that groups who experience harm find these episodes of cancel culture validating in ways that have yet to be fully explored by intergroup relations research. Further, these findings suggest that collective validation does mediate the relationship between cancel culture and collective action; thus, cancel culture becomes an important contributor to resistance by marginalized groups through collective validation.

Cancel culture involves the highly visible calling for and enacting of boycotts, condemnation, and social exiling of a person or group whose harmful behaviours or attitudes have been deemed unacceptable, offensive, or inappropriate. While predominately online, the practice of cancelling and cancel culture predates the internet and has its foundations in Black liberation and protest. Anne Charity Hudley, chair of linguistics of African America for the University of California Santa Barbara, states,

[w]hile the terminology of cancel culture may be new and most applicable to social media through Black Twitter, in particular, the concept of being canceled is not new to Black culture [… cancelling is] a survival skill as old as the Southern black use of the boycott (from Romano, 2020 ) .

She likens cancel culture to protest and boycott of people and groups, rather than businesses, and describes cancel culture as a way to empower those whose voices are marginalized; “ it’s a collective way of saying, ‘We elevated your social status, your economic prowess, [and] we are not going to pay attention to you in the way that we once did. I may have no power, but the power I have is to [ignore] you’ ” (quote from Romano, 2020 ).

However, cancel culture has been criticized by some as more problematic than helpful ( Ronson, 2016 ; Hagi, 2019 ; Romano, 2020 ; see also Drury, 2002 for reactionary crowds). Equity, diversity, and inclusion (EDI) advocate and social media influencer, Beecham (2021) argues that while the goal of cancel culture is to combat prejudice, in practice it often redirects prejudice to a new target through “us vs. them” thinking. Beecham’s argument implies a singular goal of cancel culture as combatting prejudice and claims that cancel culture is ineffective in achieving this goal. It seems hasty to claim that cancel culture’s only goal – or main goal – is to combat prejudice, and conclusions about the overall value of this social practice are premature without first considering a wider range of possible goals and impacts, especially the impacts on those who have been harmed and are speaking up. This is not to say that Beecham has neglected to consider the impacts of cancel culture on harmed groups, or that all her critiques of cancel culture are or erroneous – many have merit. Rather, we propose that cancel culture, whether effective at reducing prejudice or not, may reduce the impact of harm and/or elicit other productive forms of collective action. Harmed groups may benefit from the support, validation, and visibility from others who are involved in cancelling perpetrators of harm. That is, an episode of cancel culture might provide the conditions for the harmed group to experience feelings of collective validation that, in turn, could increase feelings of empowerment that inspire or maintain collective action.

To our knowledge, this research is the first social psychological analysis of the impact of cancel culture on collective action. In addition, research on collective action in response to intergroup harm has yet to adequately consider the role of feelings of collective validation as a motivator of collective action. The current research will begin to fill this gap and in so doing may broaden our understanding of the psychological mechanisms that inspire and maintain collective responses to intergroup harm.

Feelings of collective validation

Validation is typically understood as the recognition and affirmation of a person’s experiences and an affirmation of their feelings as legitimate. In clinical trauma therapy, practitioners have successfully used validation to support patient wellbeing and have identified feelings of validation as an important and necessary component of recovery and healing following harm (e.g., Hong and Lishner, 2016 ; Özeke-Kocabaş and Üstündağ-Budak, 2017 ). However, this research focuses on the psychopathology of trauma and practitioners’ use of trauma validation to support individual clients in interpersonal contexts. To develop the concept of intergroup validation, it may be valuable to consider group-based harm (discrimination, harassment, oppression, etc.) as trauma ( Carter, 2007 ). Thus, the concept of validation might also be useful when understanding and thinking about responses to intergroup harm.

Discussions and investigations of validation within social psychological literature are scant, but there are a few. Kalkhoff (2005) describes collective validation as occurring when “ bystanders copy or refrain from challenging a lower-status actor’s deference to a higher-status actor [or] validate deferential behavior collectively by pressuring a recalcitrant lower-status actor to defer to a higher-status counterpart ” (p. 59). More simply, Kalkhoff is suggesting that the behaviours of bystanders validate social norms within groups by pressuring lower-status members to conform or by refraining from challenging the submission of those with lower-status. However, this use of group validation focuses on intra group relations, where members of a single group validate the actions of their own group members. Contrastingly, collective validation as an intergroup phenomenon would involve the feeling that one’s group and its experiences have been recognized and validated by members of other groups. Additionally, Kalkhoff’s definition describes only how validation can further marginalize those of “lower-status” and fails to recognize that validation might also occur where a “lower-status” group challenges a “higher- status” group – members of other groups may validate the harmed group, recognize the illegitimacy of the harm, and even join them to challenge the actions of the perpetrator group and demand reparations. Therefore, we can extend Kalkhoff’s conceptualization of collective validation by examining group-based harm where the harmed group challenges the perpetrator’s behaviour and demands justice.

An interesting and relevant example of validation is present in Foster et al.’s (2021) research on women’s expectations of validation from others for engaging in online collective action. They found that when women expected greater validation for their social media activism, they showed more interest in future collective action. However, validation in Foster and colleagues’ work focused on experiences of personal validation (validation of me as a person – likable, friendly, etc.). In addition, this work focuses on expectations that one will be validated for future actions. Here, we hope to expand on this by focusing specifically on actual experienced feelings of collective validation (the feeling that one’s group is being validated).

Interestingly, collective validation of group-based harm is also briefly described in the literature on collective apologies (e.g., Hornsey and Wohl, 2013 ), where recognition of a group’s continued suffering and commitment to redress by the perpetrator group are seen as essential to an effective apology. An effective apology should offer this kind of validation. However, while a collective apology offers validation of a group’s suffering and their responses to that suffering, we propose that collective validation following harm may be sought from groups other than the perpetrators – from a wider range of agents, including ingroup members and especially members of the superordinate category (see Turner et al., 1987 ) – the larger, more inclusive group that includes the harmed group, the perpetrator group, and other groups. Thus, although the validation offered by perpetrators through apologies may be important, validation by third party groups may also be particularly valuable.

Harmed groups receiving support from a third party is also explored by Simon and Klandermans’ (2001) triangular model of politicized collective identity. This model proposes three steps that lead harmed group members to become politicized and act against harm. First, group members become aware of the harm and agree that it is unwanted (awareness of shared grievances). Next, they identify an adversary to whom they attribute the harm (adversarial attribution). If this adversary does not address their harmful behaviours, the harmed group seeks to connect with members of the broader society to gain support. Although Simon and Klandermans do not explicitly consider that the participation of third parties can serve as validation, they do consider other consequences for this recruitment of a third party, such as structuring of the understanding of the conflict as one including opponents and potential allies, rather than “ bipolar in-group/out-group confrontation ” (p. 328). This broadens the meaning of collective action to include both actions aimed at opponents (or perpetrators) and action aimed at recruiting third party allies. This implies that the contributions of other groups (third parties) are desirable and valuable to disadvantaged group members. We propose that one reason for this is that third parties offer not only a strategic advantage but also psychological validation of the ingroup and its struggle. In addition, Simon and Klandermans’ ideas map nicely onto the context of cancel culture, where members of other groups within the larger society join in the action and thus evidence themselves as potential allies with the harmed group.

Finally, most discussions of validation describe validation in terms of the actions of those who are providing validation. Thus, validation is conceptualised in terms of the behaviours of others that are intended to create the conditions for members of the harmed group to feel validated . However, these behaviours may or may not produce these feelings. Thus, any action by others is only validating to the degree that the harmed group experiences it as such. Therefore, we propose that collective validation is more aptly understood as the psychological experience of those who are targeted. Thus, the current research centers the psychological experience of the harmed group by describing and measuring collective validation as the feelings of those who have been harmed.

Cancel culture and feelings of collective validation

Okimoto and Wenzel (2008) describe how intergroup transgressions can threaten the status and power equilibrium and also threaten the validity of common values that the victim group expects are shared across the superordinate category (their community or society). The kinds of transgressions that lead to episodes of cancel culture often involve acts that threaten the victim group’s status, power and autonomy/control over their reputation (e.g., a sexist comment threatens the status and diminishes the power and autonomy of all women in that context). This is especially salient in intergroup relations where the status of the offending group may come at the expense the harmed group. Simultaneously, values the victim believes are shared broadly within society are also violated, threatening the validity of these values (e.g., the victim believes that society values women and sexist comments violate, and thus call into question, the general support of that value). According to Okimoto and Wenzel (2008) , a response to this injustice would need to address both concerns if justice is to be restored. We propose that both concerns may be partially addressed by an episode of cancel culture. When a perpetrator is “called out” for their offensive actions, they are given the opportunity to take responsibility, apologise, and restore justice. However, if the perpetrator refuses, the harmed group calls on the superordinate group – society – for justice. By isolating, humiliating, and diminishing the status of the offender, cancel culture involves the superordinate group communicating to the harmed group that their status, and autonomy over their reputation, and the importance of the shared values that had been violated by the offending group are indeed secure. Therefore, cancel culture offers to the harmed group the objective conditions that may lead to feelings of collective validation, through high visibility (e.g., amplifying the voices of harmed group members), public denouncing of social norm violation (i.e., “ we agree that the sexist comment violates a norm ”), punishment of the perpetrators and calls for justice (i.e., through cancelling and public shame), and explicit support for the harmed group (e.g., “ we believe, see, hear, and agree with you ”).

Thus, cancelling of the offending group may provide the conditions that lead members of the harmed group to feel validated through the participation of the superordinate group in reaffirming and recognizing the harmed group’s status and power, and restoring shared values in defense of the harmed group. In part this is done by increasing the visibility of the offense while simultaneously decreasing the visibility of the offender, and by increasing the visibility of the harmed group’s response to the offense, thus amplifying the voices of marginalised groups.

This process reflects Banet-Weiser’s (2018) claim that online societies function through an inequality of power, where power is embodied through visibility and attention and often politicized and commodified. Attention – in the form of likes, shares, and trending hashtags – becomes a means to promote specific online content following the same socioeconomic politics of power as the offline world. When actions challenge deeper systems of oppression and power, they become undesirable and are less likely to attract attention. Banet-Weiser explains that many popular and commodified online feminist movements, for example, fail to “ challenge deep structures of inequalities ” (p. 11) and this failure makes them more palatable to those in power and, consequently, leads them to be more visible. Additionally, “for some images and practices to become visible, others [those that do challenge deep structures of inequalities] must be rendered invisible” (p. 11). Cancelling a perpetrator group or its members can, at times, amplify and make more visible harmed and marginalized groups.

Thus, visibility may be key in inspiring feelings of collective validation as it offers public recognition and the possibility that others will also affirm the suffering caused by intergroup harm. Therefore, this research uses the context of cancel culture as one that could inspire feelings of collective validation after group-based harm because it includes the following:

  • It is provided by members of a superordinate group
  • It directly recognizes that harm was done to a particular group
  • It explicitly affirms the harmed group’s emotional responses to that harm
  • It supports and amplifies the harmed group in challenging the perpetrator group’s actions and in demanding justice
  • It has high public visibility

Even when limited and sporadic, episodes of cancel culture may have positive influences on social change through amplifying the harmed group’s voices, by temporarily canceling powerful and harmful voices, and by raising the visibility of marginalized groups. This explicit recognition and support (i.e., validation) of a harmed group may allow them to recognize their collective power (i.e., feeling of collective empowerment) and thus motivate continued efforts for social change. Therefore, we propose that one way that cancel culture can influence subsequent actions is through the resulting experience (or subjective feeling) of collective validation and its subsequent impact on feelings of collective empowerment.

Collective empowerment and collective action

We propose that experiencing validation following harm can play an important role in building the sense of collective empowerment that is critical for members of disadvantaged groups to initiate, join, and continue their involvement in collective action directed at social change (e.g., Drury and Reicher, 1999 ). Rappaport (1987) defines collective empowerment as the phenomenon or process of being able or allowed to do something because there is control or authority over that thing. Similarly, Wright (2010) conceptualises perceived collective control as comprised of two beliefs: “( 1 ) that social change is contingent upon behavior (i.e. , that the situation is modifiable ) and (2) that [ a person’s ] group in particular can execute the behaviors necessary to produce the desired change ” (p.864; see also Zimmerman and Rappaport, 1988 ).

Both representations of empowerment support the contention that collective empowerment includes two components. One is the belief that one’s group has the power to influence the social environment. However, one cannot experience these feelings of efficacy or power if one first does not first perceive the social environment as malleable to influence. That is, one must first perceive “instability” ( Tajfel and Turner, 1979 ) in the current social environment. Therefore, collective empowerment involves perceptions of instability and collective efficacy. Thus, while collective validation is the feeling that one’s group’s experiences – especially with harm – are recognised, valued, and affirmed by a superordinate group, collective empowerment is the feeling that the social environment that generated harm can be changed (instability) and that one’s group has the power and means to change it (efficacy).

Thus, if experiencing collective validation serves to heighten feelings of empowerment – then cancel culture may lead marginalized groups to engage in collective efforts to achieve social change indirectly through the psychological mechanisms of feelings of collective validation and collective empowerment.

Collective emotions and collective action

In addition to recognizing and condemning the harm done to the group, cancel culture might also offer legitimacy to collective emotions such as anger and contempt. Thus, the feeling of validation that results from an episode of cancel culture could also include the sense that one’s strong negative emotions about perpetrators of harm are appropriate, which in turn may strengthen the expression of emotions that are well-documented to provide one the psychological foundation for collective action – outgroup-directed anger and contempt. For example, Stürmer and Simon (2009) found that anger was a direct predictor of collective action as it provided an avenue for participants to relieve aggressive tension. Similarly, van Zomeren et al. (2008a) identify group-based anger as a unique pathway to collective action (see also Tausch et al., 2011 ; van Zomeren et al., 2012 ; van Zomeren, 2013 ) describing how “emotional social support validates the group-based appraisal of [an] event, which also affirms emotional responses like anger” ( van Zomeren et al., 2004 , p. 650). However, few of these studies focus on the combination of anger and contempt.

Anger and contempt are distinct, yet highly related emotions. Mackie et al. (2000) argue that anger – which is the emotional evaluation of someone’s actions – is related to “action tendencies against the triggering agent” (p. 610) or attack behaviours (e.g., violence, arguing). Contempt is the emotional evaluation of someone’s worth and is related to exclusionary actions (e.g., avoidance or exiling behaviour). Groups that are traditionally marginalized by oppressive systems may feel contempt for perpetrator groups following incidents of harm because of the long history of attempts to address injustices with little success. Simultaneously, these harmed groups might still hope for an end to oppression – which requires cooperation from perpetrator groups – and, thus, being angry or frustrated with the perpetrator groups for their lack of cooperation in resolving conflict.

Evident in most episodes of cancel culture is the presence of both anger and contempt where participants engage in both attack and exclusionary actions simultaneously. The act of attacking someone’s reputation is an anger response. Social exiling of perpetrators is a contempt response to unacceptable behaviour. This mixture of anger and contempt make cancel culture an interesting example of intergroup conflict.

Further, the participation of superordinate group members in cancelling of the perpetrators may communicate to the harmed group that their emotional responses of both anger and contempt are reasonable, merited, and valid. The resulting feeling of validation of one’s anger and contempt on behalf of their group’s experience may serve to increase these emotions, which should in turn increase their likelihood of engaging in collective action. Therefore, this current research will assess whether the subjective feelings of validation that emerges as a result of an episode of cancel culture also enhance anger and contempt which should lead to greater willingness to engage in collective action.

Theoretical model

This theorizing results in a sequential mediational model that proposes that, following collective harm, an episode of cancel culture should elicit feelings of collective validation in the harmed group. This experience of collective validation strengthens feeling of both collective empowerment and the collective emotions of anger and contempt, which in turn lead to stronger intentions to engage in collective action. Thus, it is predicted that an episode of cancel culture will have indirect positive impacts on collective anger and contempt, feelings of collective empowerment, and collective action that are mediated by feelings of collective validation.

The current research

Two experimental studies test the model presented in Figure 1 . Study 1 uses a university student sample and focuses on the responses of women to a blatantly sexist incident on campus. Study 2 uses a community sample of East Asian Canadians and Americans and focuses on an episode of cancel culture in response to an act of anti-East Asian discrimination.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-14-1181872-g001.jpg

Theoretical mediation model.

In both studies it is expected that being exposed to an episode of cancel culture, versus a control condition, will increase collective action intentions and behaviours. This effect will be sequentially mediated first by feelings of collective validation, followed by both collective anger and contempt and collective empowerment (see Figure 1 ). We do not have a priori predictions about a direct pathway between the manipulation of cancel culture and collective anger and contempt or between the manipulation of cancel culture and collective empowerment.

Study 1: sexism and university women

Participants.

Data were collected between October 2021 and March 2022. Cases were removed if participants did not complete the survey (133 cases), spent less than 500 s (approximately 8 min; 17 cases), or spent more than 7,200 s (120 min; 65 cases) on the survey. 1 Finally, repeating cases were removed (20 cases). The final sample, after removing problematic cases (e.g., nonconsenting, repeated submissions, etc.), consisted of 520 university women. Demographic information is listed in Table 1 .

Summary of participant demographics (Study 1).

Race/ethnicityParticipants ( = 520)
%
Black or African91.7
East Asian13325.6
Indigenous61.2
Latino/a/e or Hispanic122.3
Middle Eastern/North African346.5
South Asian11622.3
South-East Asian5410.4
White or European19236.9
Prefer to specify or provide more detail193.7
Age %
18–2043383.3
21–258215.8
26–3010.2
31–3510.2
36–4010.2
40+20.4

University women were recruited online using the Psychology Department’s Research Participation System (RPS). Participants were told that the researchers were “ interested in understanding perceptions of community responses to contentious incidents and topics in campus environments .” Those who signed up for the study were randomly assigned to one of two conditions. In both conditions, participants read a fictitious scenario of a sexist incident on campus. In the cancel culture condition this incident was followed by a description of a highly visible episode of cancel culture against the perpetrator group. In the control condition, the incident was followed by some filler/neutral information about unrelated activities on campus.

Following exposure to the given condition, participants responded to measures of feelings of collective validation, collective empowerment, anger, contempt, and collective action intentions.

Cancel culture condition

The cancel culture condition included a scenario describing a sexist incident on campus (modeled on a real event at Texas Tech University; see Barbato, 2014 ; Servantes, 2014 ) followed by a description of a targeted campaign by others from the community to cancel a fictitious fraternity (e.g., #EndEtaNu). The episode of cancel culture includes components naturally found in real-world cancel culture scenarios such as high visibility (“ […] the post had 200 likes and has been shared over 30 times by the university community and beyond ”), public denouncing of norm violation (“ women on campus have every right to be pissed ”), punishment of the perpetrators (“ Two Eta Nu members […] fired from a co-op position and […] suspended from the swim team ”) and calls for justice (“ A petition […] calling for the removal of Eta Nu chapter and already has over 1,500 signatures in less than a week ”), and objective support of the harmed group (“ this is hurtful and has very real consequences for women on campus ”).

Control condition

The control condition included the sexist incident on campus scenario, but the description of the campaign to cancel the fraternity was replaced with filler information about other homecoming incidents unrelated to the sexist incident or to issues of gender or sexism more generally (e.g., vandalism and littering, a student getting stuck on top of a residence building).

This measure was constructed for this study.

Participants responded to 24 items consistent with the definition of subjective or felt collective validation previously outlined (e.g., “ Experiences of harm faced by women are recognized by the university community ”) on a 7-point scale (1 Strongly disagree to 7 Strongly agree ). Higher scores indicate greater felt collective validation ( α =0 .79). See Appendix A . 2

Collective anger and contempt

Participants responded to 8 items measuring anger and contempt ( Mackie et al., 2000 ). They reported the extent to which they felt angry, displeased, irritated, furious, contemptuous, disgusted, repelled, and sick on 5-point scales (1 Not at all to 5 Extremely ). Higher scores indicate greater reported feelings of anger and contempt ( α =0.94). While Mackie et al. use two separate measures for anger and contempt, this study uses one since Mackie et al. found a high correlation between the two emotions, which was mirrored in these data ( r = 0.86).

Collective empowerment

Our initial conceptualization of empowerment research included perceived instability and collective efficacy. However, our 3-item measure of perceived instability of the relationship between the perpetrator group and harmed group adapted from Mummendey et al. (1999) and Wright et al. (2020) had very low reliability ( α = 0.56) and including it undermined the reliability of our measure of collective empowerment. Thus, these three items were not included, and the overall final measure of collective empowerment included two scales.

The second was a 5-item measure of collective efficacy adapted from Sabherwal et al. (2021) . Participants rated how likely “ Women on campus, working together, can influence the following groups to do something about gender-based violence on campus ”: (a) the federal government, (b) the provincial government, (c) the local government, (d) university administrators, and (e) fraternity leaders on 7-point scales (1 Not at all to 7 Extremely ). Higher scores indicate greater perceived collective efficacy ( α = 0.85).

The third measure was a more general measure of empowerment that included 4 items measuring women’s feelings of strength, control, and power (e.g., “ I feel that women on campus are strong ”) on a 7-point agreement scale (1 Strongly disagree to 7 Strongly agree ). Higher scores indicate greater feelings of empowerment ( α = 0.81).

The collective efficacy and general empowerment measures were aggregated to provide a single collective empowerment score with higher scores indicating greater feelings of empowerment ( α = 0.80).

Collective action intentions

Participants responded to 10 items measuring their willingness to participate in various forms of anti-sexism activism (e.g., “ I would participate in a rally demanding equal salaries for men and women ”; “ I would act against sexism in general ”) on 7-point scales (1 Very unlikely to 7 Very likely ). This measure was adapted from Becker and Wright (2011) for use in a 2022 Canadian Context (e.g., “ I would donate for a women’s organization which lobbies for women’s rights, such as Terres des femmes ” was changed to “ I would donate to an organization that advocates for women’s rights, such as Canadian Women’s Foundation ”). Higher scores indicate stronger intentions to participate in collective action ( α = 0.92).

Predicted mediational analyses

The hypothesised model using the measure of Collective Action Intentions as the dependent variable was assessed using SPSS PROCESS Model 81 ( Hayes, 2022 ).

Condition (cancel culture vs. control) had a significant direct effect on Feelings of Collective Validation, which also had a significant direct effect on Collective Empowerment. Collective Empowerment had a significant direct effect on Collective Action Intention. The direct effect of Condition on Collective Action Intentions was not significant, and the only significant indirect effect of Condition on Collective Action Intention was a sequential mediation through both Feelings of Collective Validation and Collective Empowerment [ β = 0.03, 95% CI (0.01, 0.05)]. The causal steps approach to mediational analyses (see Baron and Kenny, 1986 ) held that a direct effect of the independent variable on the final outcome variable (or the total effect) should be a “gatekeeper” to mediational analyses. However, more recent statisticians have argued against this approach ( Shrout and Bolger, 2002 ; Hayes, 2009 ) and have suggested using bootstrapping to test for indirect effects ( MacKinnon et al., 2004 ). This bootstrapping method is the approach taken in Hayes SPSS PROCESS Macro ( Hayes, 2022 ) that was used for these analyses. Therefore, these results suggest the need to consider other possible pathways between Condition and Collective Action Intentions that were not included in this model ( Table 2 ).

Correlation matrix for all variables.

Mean (SD)ConditionCollective validationCollective emotionsCollective empowerment
Collective validation4.3 (0.7)0.18
Collective emotions4.4 (0.7)0.060.01
Collective empowerment4.6 (1.0)0.09 0.27 −0.06
Collective action intentions5.6 (1.2)−0.010.11 0.26 0.28

The predicted direct effect of Feelings of Collective Validation on Collective Emotions was non-significant and thus the sequential indirect effect of Condition on Collective Action Intentions mediated by Feelings of Collective Validation and Collective Emotions was non-significant ( β = −0.00, 95% CI (−0.01, 0.01)). However, the direct effect of Collective Emotions on Collective Action Intentions was significant. See Table 3 and Figure 2 for details of these direct effects.

Direct effects of predicted model 81.

Feelings of Collective Validation
95% CI
Condition
Feelings of collective validation
Collective anger and contempt
Collective empowerment
Constant3.920.09<0.0013.744.10
= 0.03
(1,518) = 16.28, < 0.001
Collective anger and contempt
95% CI
Condition0.090.060.15−0.030.22
Feelings of collective validation−0.010.050.92−0.100.90
Collective anger and contempt
Collective empowerment
Constant4.330.21<0.0013.904.73
= 0.00
(2,517) = 1.03, = 0.36
Collective empowerment
95% CI
Condition0.090.090.32−0.080.26
Feelings of collective validation
Collective anger and contempt
Collective empowerment
Constant2.810.29<0.0012.243.38
= 0.07
(2,517) = 20.60, < 0.001
Collective action intent
95% CI
Condition−0.130.100.20−0.320.07
Feelings of collective validation0.100.080.32−0.070.23
Collective anger and contempt
Collective empowerment
Constant2.210.45<0.0011.323.10
= 0.14
(4,515) = 21.74, < 0.001

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-14-1181872-g002.jpg

Study 1 mediation model result. Partial support for the predicted sequential indirect effects of condition on collective action intention mediated by feelings of collective validation, collective anger and contempt, collective empowerment. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.01.

This first experimental study provides initial evidence that being exposed to an episode of cancel culture does elicit feelings of collective validation and these feelings are associated with a stronger sense of collective empowerment which in turn is associated with greater collective action intention. To our knowledge, this is the first time that a positive relationship between exposure to cancel culture and intentions to engage in collective action has been demonstrated using an experimental design. However, the correlation between Condition and Collective Action Intentions is small (−0.01), implying the possible presence of other pathways in the total model. Regardless, despite legitimate critiques of cancel culture and a recognition that, like cancel culture itself, these effects may be limited in time and scope, it does appear cancel culture in response to a blatant act of sexism can have a positive impact on women’s interest in taking actions to fight gender inequality.

Collective empowerment and feelings of collective validation

While collective – and personal – empowerment has been linked to greater collective action in previous work ( Drury and Reicher, 1999 ; van Zomeren et al., 2008b ; Wright, 2010 ), this research provides preliminary support for the role of collective validation as a precursor for women’s sense of collective empowerment and, thus, intentions to participate in collective action. Therefore, future research on motivating factors for collective action may benefit from the inclusion of feelings of collective validation and consider sources of this validation (e.g., cancel culture). It is also possible that instances of allyship such as cancel culture elicit collective validation of a group’s existing power through communicating collective care and, thus, group value (i.e., “ we care about your wellbeing and experiences with harm because you matter ”; e.g., Smith, 1997 ; Weis et al., 2006 ; Bradbury-Jones et al., 2011 ). This is not to say that cancel culture should be used to motivate groups toward collective action, rather it serves as an interesting and important example of allyship from a superordinate group that may, through collective validation (i.e., recognition, support, and increased visibility) of a harmed group, foster the collective empowerment needed to motivate groups toward collective action.

Emotions and feelings of collective validation

Consistent with previous research (e.g., Stürmer and Simon, 2009 ; Tausch et al., 2011 ; Shepherd and Evans, 2020 ), stronger feelings of anger and contempt were associated with stronger collective action intentions. However, this relationship seems to be separate from cancel culture and feelings of collective validation. One may argue that the collective validation that emerges from cancel culture may alleviate feelings of anger and contempt. However, the women in the current study were substantially angry and contemptuous ( x ̄ =4.43, SD = 0.71, on a 5-point scale). Hence, two other possible explanations for this finding are that (1) emotions constitute a pathway toward collective action that is separate from collective validation and that these emotions are more influenced by the nature of the harmful/unjust actions of the perpetrator group than by the subsequent responses to those actions and (2) there is a difference between affective social support and instrumental social support present in this research.

van Zomeren et al. (2008a) social identity model of collective action (SIMCA) proposes three distinct predictors of collective action: affective justice (emotional responses to perceived injustice – such as anger); politicized identity; and collective efficacy. Therefore, it is possible that collective anger and contempt are motivating collective action intention for reasons separate from feelings of collective validation.

In addition, according to van Zomeren et al. (2004) , emotional social support (support of the harmed group’s emotions and opinions) influenced collective action through collective anger, whereas instrumental social support (direct action by a superordinate group to correct the injustice) influenced collective action through collective efficacy (see also van Zomeren et al., 2008a affective vs. nonaffective injustice). Therefore, it is also possible that the cancel culture scenario presented in the current research elicited a sense of instrumental social support more so than emotional social support resulting in the lack of a relationship between feelings of collective validation and collective anger and contempt. For example, the campus community petitioning administration to expel the perpetrators and enact consequences for the Eta Nu fraternity may have been viewed by participants as instrumental collective validation (or social support) since these examples represent public actions.

Improvements for study 2

Study 2 will extend the current findings with three important changes from Study 1. First, we will test the generality of the findings by focusing on a different intergroup relationship. Thus, Study 2, includes a community sample of East Asian Canadians and Americans and focuses on responses to an incident of Anti-Asian discrimination. Second, in order to better consider the impact of emotions as mediators, the scenario presenting the manipulation will include more messages that reflect affective responses ( van Zomeren et al., 2004 ). Third, to streamline the data collection, the 2-part measure of collective empowerment used in Study 1 will be replaced by a simpler, more direct measure ( Moya-Garófano et al., 2021 ).

Study 2: anti-East Asian racism

Incidents of violence and anti-East Asian hate crimes are on the rise following the 2020 COVID-19 outbreaks ( Chen et al., 2020 ). In addition, the East Asian diaspora is beginning to speak openly about this xenophobia and racism. Prior to these recent events, racism toward East Asian Canadians and Americans has typically been marginalised and downplayed, at times even by East Asian communities (e.g., Tai, 2020 ; Shao and Lin, 2021 ). This may be in part due to the Model Minority Myth (MMM) that minimises and marginalizes East Asian communities and groups’ experiences with racism since they are viewed as passive, privileged, successful and even as “honorary whites” who do not complain or protest like other systemically marginalised groups ( Aguirre and Lio, 2008 ; Yoo et al., 2010 ; Lee, 2022 ). Thus, East Asian peoples may fear that drawing attention to themselves via collective action against anti-East Asian racism might result in backlash and more racist discrimination, rather than support or solidarity, from the dominant groups that endorse the MMM. Wei et al. (2010) found that Asian American women who endorsed direct confrontations of gender discrimination reported more negative outcomes including decreased life satisfaction. While the MMM has received considerable academic and public critique, it remains a common representation of East Asian communities in both the US and Canada. The resulting lack of public attention has also led to limited attention from social psychological and anti-racism research. However, anti-Asian racism has been rampant in North America for decades, and there is growing public discourse and activism ( Brockell, 2021 ; Government of Canada, 2021 ; Lee, 2022 ). Therefore, a focus on anti- East Asian discrimination and collective responses to it are both important and timely. Thus, we will use this context to explore the role of cancel culture, feelings of collective validation, emotions, and collective empowerment in motivating collective action.

Focus group

Prior to beginning the study, the second author conducted a focus group with East Asian Canadian students. The rationale was to include members of East Asian communities early in the research process as we were developing the content of the manipulations and adapting the measures to this new intergroup context. It was important that the scenarios and measures be respectful of East Asian cultures, perspectives, experiences, and communities. The specific goal of the focus group was to gauge how East Asian students responded to an early draft of the scenarios used in the manipulation and to use their feedback to make alterations. In addition, it was important that the list of potential collective actions was culturally relevant (e.g., would members of East Asian communities be likely to attend a protest).

In the focus group, the students read both the control and cancel culture fictitious racism scenarios and the collective action intention measure and provided feedback on each. No changes were made to the fictitious scenarios as a result of the focus group feedback. We received positive feedback from participants about the realism, cultural sensitivity, and participant responses to both the control and cancel culture conditions. However, the collective action measure was modified to include some actions in which the group indicated members of East Asian communities would be more likely to engage (e.g., writing to government officials).

Data was collected via Prolific , an online survey research platform for social sciences. The survey was available to participants who were of East Asian descent and residing in either Canada or the United States. Participants were remunerated with $6 USD for approximately 30 min of time. Cases were removed if participants did not complete the core measures of the survey (33 cases) or spent less than 500 s (approximately 8 min; 12 cases). 3 Despite using Prolific demographic filters, some participants did not indicate being of East Asian descent and these cases were removed (17 cases). Due to a technological error, the randomly assigned condition for two participants was not recorded by Qualtrics and these cases were removed. The total sample following removal of problematic cases consisted of 237 self-identified East Asian Canadian and American participants. Demographic information is listed in Table 4 below.

Summary of participant demographics (Study 2).

Country of residenceParticipants ( = 237)
%
Canada7230.4
The USA16569.6
East Asian Ethnic Group %
Chinese14561.2
Japanese198
Korean3916.5
Mongolian00
Taiwanese166.8
Tibetan10.4
Vietnamese145.9
Prefer to specify or provide more detail135.5
Parent East Asian ethnic group %
Chinese14862.4
Japanese198
Korean3715.6
Mongolian00
Taiwanese166.8
Tibetan10.4
Vietnamese114.6
Prefer to specify or provide more detail177.2
Grandparent East Asian ethnic group %
Chinese15063.3
Japanese218.9
Korean3816
Mongolian00
Taiwanese166.8
Tibetan10.4
Vietnamese93.8
Prefer to specify or provide more detail145.9
Disability %
An autism spectrum disorder73
A chronic health condition114.6
A communication impairment00
A developmental disability00
A learning disability41.7
A mental illness or health challenge239.7
A mobility or orthopedic impairment31.3
A sensory impairment20.8
A temporary impairment due to illness or injury31.3
Prefer to specify or provide more detail10.4
Prefer not to answer198
Not applicable18477.6
Gender %
Woman (cisgender and transgender)11347.9
Agender20.8
Non-binary/gender fluid/gender non-conforming/gender queer20.8
Man (cisgender and transgender)11247.5
Prefer not to answer62.6
Sexual orientation %
Asexual104.2
Bisexual166.8
Gay31.3
Lesbian41.7
Pansexual10.4
Heterosexual/straight19281
Queer20.8
Prefer to self-describe10.4
Prefer not to answer83.4
Age (years) %
18–246828.7
25–348837.1
35–444016.9
45–542410.1
55–64114.6
65–7410.4
Prefer not to answer52.1
Education (highest level completed) %
High school diploma166.8
Some college3414.3
Undergraduate/Bachelor’s degree13958.6
Graduate/Master’s degree3615.2
Doctorate/Professional degree93.8
Prefer not to answer31.3
Income (yearly, pre-tax) %
< $10,0002611
$10,000–$19,999114.6
$20,000–$29,999166.8
$30,000–$39,999177.2
$40,000–$49,999218.9
$50,000–$59,999187.6
$60,000–$69,999135.5
$70,000–$79,9992510.6
$80,000–$89,999135.4
$90,000–$99,999145.9
$100,000–$149,999218.9
> $150,000218.9
Prefer not to answer208.5
Employment status %
Employed full time13155.3
Employed part time3414.3
Unemployed looking for work208.4
Unemployed not looking for work10.4
Retired20.8
Student4016.9
Prefer not to answer93.8
Residence %
Owned or being bought11347.9
Rented for money6728.4
Occupied without payment or money or rent20.8
Student housing provided by university41.7
Living with friends20.8
Living with family4820.3

In this pre-registered experiment, 4 Participants were told that the researchers were interested in “understanding responses to contentious online topics and behaviours.” Those who signed up for the study were randomly assigned to one of two conditions. In both conditions, participants read a fictitious scenario of a racist incident in the Vancouver community. In the cancel culture condition this was followed by a description of an episode of cancel culture against the perpetrator group. In the control condition, participants read only the scenario of a racist incident without any additional information. Following exposure to the given condition, participants responded to measures of the same variables in Study 1.

Manipulation

The cancel culture condition included a scenario describing a racist incident in the community followed by a description of a targeted campaign by others from the community to cancel the perpetrator. While the racist incident described in the scenario was fictitious, it was based on real experiences of East Asian and Pacific Islander Canadians and Americans throughout the Covid-19 pandemic (e.g., Kambhampaty and Sakaguchi, 2020 ; Baylon and Cecco, 2021 ). Descriptions of these real-world experiences were reviewed prior to writing the scenario. The episode of cancel culture included components naturally found in real-world cancel culture scenarios such as high visibility (“ the post had 2,200 likes and has been shared over 300 times by the Vancouver community and beyond ”); public denouncing of norm violation (“ This is deplorable, especially during a time when we should be working together ”); punishment of the perpetrators (e.g., calls for police investigations and expulsion from programs) and calls for justice (“ It’s time for racists in this community to face the consequences of their actions ”); and expressions of support of the harmed group (“ East-Asian people are right to expect better from their neighbors ”).

Control scenario

The control condition included the same racist incident but did not include the description of the cancel culture episode.

Participants responded to 24-items consistent with the definition of feelings of collective validation (e.g., “ The experiences of harm faced by East Asian people are recognized by the community ”). Responses were provided on a 5-point scale (1 Strongly disagree to 5 Strongly agree ). Higher scores indicate greater felt collective validation ( α = 0.91). See Appendix A . 5

Participants responded to a shorter (4-item rather than 8-item) version of the anger (anger, frustration) and contempt (contemptuous and disgusted) measure adapted from Mackie et al. (2000) . Responses were provided on a 5-point scale (1 Not at all to 5 Extremely ). Higher scores indicate greater anger and contempt ( α = 0.90).

Participants responded to a 10-item measure that asked “ As an East Asian person, if I were in the situation described in the article, I would feel ”: empowered, in control of the situation, humiliated, inferior, defenseless, full of energy, stimulated, independent, not in control of the situation, and weak ( Moya-Garófano et al., 2021 ). Because of a technical error in the Qualtrics file, five of the 10 items (full of energy, stimulated, independent, not in control of the situation, and weak) were randomly omitted for 80 participants. Thus, a shorter 5-item version of the measure was used to maintain the entire sample. Responses were provided on a 5-point scale (1 Not at all to 5 Extremely ), and the reliability for this 5-item version was good ( α = 0.82; full 10-item measure α = 0.89). Scores for items indicating helplessness/disempowerment were reversed so that higher overall scores indicate greater feelings of collective empowerment.

Participants respond to 10-items about their willingness to participate in various forms of activism against anti-East Asian racism (e.g., “ Write a letter/email to government officials in my area regarding policies that impact East Asian peoples and cultures ”) on a 5-point scale (1 Very unlikely to 5 Very likely ). This measure was adapted from Becker and Wright (2011) for use with East Asian participants but was also informed by the focus group feedback with East Asian Canadian students regarding racism in their communities and ways they would be (un)likely to respond. Higher scores indicate greater willingness/intention to participate in collective action ( α = 0.90).

The results show a significant positive direct effect of Condition on Feelings of Collective Validation, but also a significant direct effect of Condition on Collective Empowerment that was not predicted. As predicted, there was a significant direct effect of Feelings of Collective Validation on Collective Empowerment. However, there was also a significant direct effect of Feelings of Collective Validation on Collective Anger and Contempt. In addition, there was a significant negative direct effect of Condition on Collective Anger and Contempt (not mediated by Feelings of Collective Validation) that was not predicted.

Further, Collective Anger and Contempt had a significant positive effect on Collective Action Intention, but the effect of Collective Empowerment on Collective Action Intention found in Study 1 did not emerge here.

Finally, significant indirect effects were found for Condition on Collective Action Intention mediated by Collective Anger and Contempt ( β = −0.16, 95% CI (−0.32, −0.04)), and for Condition on Collective Action Intention mediated sequentially by Collective Validation and Collective Anger and Contempt, and [ β = 0.09, 95% CI (0.02, 0.20)]. See Tables 5 , ​ ,6 6 and Figure 3 below for details.

Mean (SD)ConditionFeelings of collective validationCollective anger and contemptCollective empowerment
Feelings of collective validation3.39 (0.66)0.566**
Collective anger and contempt4.26 (0.82)−0.0920.097
Collective empowerment2.33 (0.91)0.274**0.302**−0.382**
Collective action intentions3.36 (0.89)−0.0250.0560.439**−0.186**

*Correlation significant at the 0.05 level.

**Correlation significant at the 0.01 level.

Feelings of collective validation
95% CI
Condition
Feelings of collective validation
Collective anger and contempt
Collective empowerment
Constant2.270.11<0.0012.042.49
= 0.32
(1,235) = 110.76, < 0.01
Collective anger and contempt
95% CI
Condition
Feelings of collective validation
Collective anger and contempt
Collective empowerment
Constant3.860.28<0.0013.314.41
= 0.04
(2, 234) = 5.05, < 0.01
Collective empowerment
95% CI
Condition
Feelings of collective validation
Collective anger and contempt
Collective empowerment
Constant0.900.30<0.0010.311.48
= 0.11
(2, 234) = 13.96, < 0.001
Collective action intent
95% CI
Condition0.020.130.85−0.230.28
Feelings of collective validation0.020.100.83−0.180.22
Collective anger and contempt
Collective empowerment−0.030.070.65−0.160.10
Constant1.370.41<0.0010.562.81
= 0.19
(4, 232) = 13.95, < 0.001

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-14-1181872-g003.jpg

Study 2 mediation model result. Partial support for the predicted sequential indirect effects of condition on collective action intention mediated by feelings of collective validation, collective anger and contempt. The direct effects of condition on collective anger and contempt and on collective empowerment were unexpected. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.01.

Overall, there is support for the primary prediction that being exposed to an episode of cancel culture will elicit feelings of collective validation and that these feelings of collective validation will be positively associated with collective action intentions. However, the overall correlation between Condition and Collective Action Intentions was small, suggesting possible other variables in the total effect.

Further, it was collective validation’s positive influence on collective anger and contempt, rather than empowerment, that accounted for its positive association with collective action intentions. In fact, while exposure to an episode of cancel culture and the subsequent feelings of collective validation were associated with collective empowerment, this empowerment did not enhance collective action intentions.

The significant association between Feelings of Collective Validation and Collective Anger and Contempt found in Study 2 is consistent with van Zomeren et al.’s (2004) work on affective social support versus instrumental social support. The description of the cancel culture episode in Study 2 included more evidence of shared affective experiences (e.g., it included more emotionally charged comments such as, “If this bigot loses his business because of this, so be it ”), and this evidence of shared emotions appears to have served to validate the participants’ own feelings of collective anger and contempt. Thus, this aspect of validation may be similar to what van Zomeren et al. (2004) are describing as affective social support, which they have shown increases interest in collective action through increased anger (e.g., van Zomeren et al., 2008b ). Thus, it appears that cancel culture can serve to collectively validate action-oriented negative emotions like anger and contempt if the content and tone of cancelling focuses on these shared emotions. Thus, collective validation may lead to collective action through either what van Zomeren et al. (2004) have called an emotional (usually anger; see also van Zomeren et al., 2008a ) pathway or the collective empowerment pathway shown in Study 1 – or perhaps in some cases through both pathways.

Further, there was an interesting and unexpected negative direct effect of Condition on Collective Anger and Contempt and negative indirect effect of Condition on Collective Action Intentions mediated by Collective Anger and Contempt. These findings may indicate that for our East Asian participants, exposure to cancel culture in defense of their group may alleviate perpetrator-directed negative emotions such as anger and contempt, rather than increase them. Perhaps this is because, through engaging in cancel culture, the superordinate group shares the emotional burden of anger and contempt with the harmed group, especially if affective social support is present. However, these findings also emphasise the importance of the role of collective validation in the model. More research is needed to understand these effects fully.

Collective empowerment and collective action intentions

The lack of a significant effect of empowerment on collective action intention is inconsistent with the findings of previous research and theorizing (e.g., Drury et al., 2005 ), as well as the surprising significant negative collective validation and empowerment pathway in the exploratory analysis. There are several possible explanations for these findings. The measure of collective empowerment only included five of the 10 items because of a random error. It is also possible that the measure did not capture the aspects of collective empowerment that drive collective action.

Construct validity of collective empowerment measure

The Collective Empowerment measure may be measuring something other than empowerment as it is usually defined and understood. Items on this empowerment measure such as “ humiliated ,” “ inferior ,” and “ defenseless ” may be capturing feelings of safety and group status instead of empowerment ( Hartling and Luchetta, 1999 ; Edmondson, 2004 ; Farbod et al., 2017 ) that may lead participants to disengage in response to harmful situations to protect themselves and their communities from further harm or backlash.

To roughly assess the construct validity of the measure, Model 81 was reanalyzed using only the “ empowerment” and “ in control of the situation” items of the Collective Empowerment measure (α = 0.86). Both items include components of empowerment as defined by Rappaport (1987) and Wright (2010) and are consistent with the empowerment measure used in Study 1. Overall, participants reported low Collective Empowerment ( x ̄ =1.95, SD = 0.95 on a 5-point scale). However, results show a significant, but small, positive direct effect of Collective Empowerment on Collective Action Intentions ( β = 0.12, SE = 0.06, 95% CI (0.00, 0.25), p = 0.05), with small positive significant indirect effects of Condition on Collective Action Intentions mediated by Collective Empowerment ( β = 0.07, 95% CI (0.00, 0.17)) and of Condition on Collective Action Intentions mediated sequentially by Feelings of Collective Validation and Collective Empowerment ( β = 0.03, 95% CI (0.00, 0.06)). Thus, while the effects are small, there is evidence that the negative items on the Moya-Garófano et al. (2021) empowerment measure may be impacting the construct validity of the measure.

Feelings of collective validation and social identity

The social identity of East Asian peoples might be important for understanding the pathway from collective validation to collective action. Indeed, some prior research has identified social identity as an important and meaningful predictor of collective action. For example, van Zomeren et al.’s (2008a) SIMCA includes politicized identity as a main predictive pathway of collective action (see also Simon and Klandermans, 2001 ). Similarly, Foster et al. (2021) found politicized identity to be important for women engaging in online collective action. Thus, it is possible that the scenario in each study primed participants to think more about their woman (Study 1) or East Asian (Study 2) identities and this may be responsible for the differing empowerment and collective anger and contempt results.

Further, most of the literature on empowerment and collective action focuses on White Western samples and contexts (see Zimmerman, 2000 ; Lardier et al., 2020 ), which may not generalize to the East Asian Canadians/Americans in this sample. Therefore, it is possible that episodes of cancel culture that explicitly provide greater affective social support of the East Asian community (as was done in Study 2), may inspire the kind of collective validation that allows for greater expression of perpetrator-directed negative emotions. This may be, in part, because this affective social support directly challenges stereotypical expectations of East Asian people imposed by the MMM ( Aguirre and Lio, 2008 ; Yoo et al., 2010 ; Lee, 2022 ).

Therefore, future research should consider the important role of specific social identities and the nature of the existing intergroup relations experienced by those groups. Feelings of collective validation and its impact on emotions and collective empowerment may vary depending on the specific histories, and current social realities of these different groups.

As well, it should be noted that the cancel culture scenarios in both studies were based on real-world instances and widely understood definitions of cancel culture. For example, Ng (2020) states that cancel culture is “ the withdrawal of any kind of support (viewership, social media follows, purchases of products endorsed by the person, etc.) for those who are assessed to have said or done something unacceptable or highly problematic, generally from a social justice perspective especially alert to sexism, heterosexism, homophobia, racism, bullying, and related issues ” (p. 623). The scenarios used in the current studies reflect this definition. However, it must also be recognized that no direct manipulation checks were used to assess whether participants perceived the scenarios to reflect or represent an episode of cancel culture. Thus, future studies should address this potential limitation by including manipulation checks or providing other direct evidence that participants recognized the scenarios as reflecting the core elements of cancel culture.

General discussion

Two experimental studies examined the role of feelings of collective validation in the context of cancel culture as an important determinant of collective action through its impact on collective empowerment and collective anger and contempt. Across both studies, there is evidence that feelings of collective validation play an important mediating role in the relationship between cancel culture and collective action intentions. However, the two studies provide a somewhat less definitive story concerning the mediational processes that account for collective validation’s association with collective action. Study 1 supports only a collective empowerment pathway from collective validation to collective action intentions in a sexism context, while Study 2 supports only a collective anger and contempt pathway from collective validation to collective action intentions in an anti-East Asian racism context.

Implications and future directions

Cancel culture, feelings of collective validation, and collective action intentions.

Overall, the present research provides novel evidence that cancel culture and feelings of collective validation should be included and examined in collective action research and theory. For example, through cancel culture, members of the superordinate group – which includes more than just members of the relevant ingroup and members of the perpetrator group – can be involved in challenging a perpetrator group’s actions and disrupting (however fleeting) the online economy of visibility and structures of inequalities. This support from other members of the superordinate group can be validating for members of the group that has been harmed and this validation can agitate them enough to challenge the individuals, groups, and systems that have perpetuated this harm.

These findings are consistent with a recent study by Foster et al. (2021 , see Study 2) who found that women were motivated toward collective action when they anticipated greater personal validation from others for responding to sexist tweets. Similarly, Droogendyk et al. (2016) identify “supportive contact” as an important determinant of increased collective action by the harmed group. This concept, in which an advantaged group member explicitly expresses their opposition to inequality and supports the harmed group’s goals, coincides with the elements of cancel culture (explicitly acknowledging harm and supporting group goals). Thus, it is possible that supportive contact elicits feelings of collective validation in similar ways as cancel culture and including measures of collective validation in future work on supportive contact may offer insights into the psychological mechanisms involved in its influences on collective action.

Cancel culture, collective validation, and Allyship

The present research also supports the inclusion of cancel culture and feelings of collective validation in research on allyship, as they appear to encompass important components of allyship. Along with supportive contact, Becker et al.’s (2022) (see also Becker and Wright, 2022) concept of “politicized contact” seems to be relevant to participation in cancel culture. In their work, Becker and colleagues show that contact that recognizes and includes discussion of group inequality is linked to greater solidarity-based allyship behaviour by advantaged group members. Thus, it seems that when members of the advantaged group, the harmed group, and even third-party groups all jointly engage, cancel culture could serve as a proxy for politicized contact and thus may increase solidarity-based collective action intentions among all three groups.

However, a critique of cancel culture is that it can backfire and alienate allies by making them afraid of being cancelled themselves for making simple mistakes. Ross (2021) claims “ [i]n our pursuit of political purity, we are alienating a lot of our allies, and we are criticizing them for not being ‘woke’ enough .” To address this strain on the ally-ingroup relationship, Ross promotes “call in” culture where allies and group members can have open, non-judgmental conversations about harm. Ross claims that call-in culture is about “ achieving accountability with grace, love, and respect as opposed to anger, shame, and humiliation .” This “call in” approach shares much with Becker et al.’s (2022) description of politicized contact and thus these conversations may well serve to increase allyship behaviours among the advantaged group.

However, the issue with positioning call-in culture and cancel culture against each other is that the goals and motivations of these two practices differ substantially. The goal for call-in culture, according to Ross, is to end oppression through meaningful work with allies, while one goal of cancel culture is to hold accountable powerful and perpetrator groups and people who refuse to hold themselves accountable. While calling someone in might be helpful with a willing and open ally, what happens when calling in fails because the harmful party refuses to acknowledge the harm they have caused? Who holds them responsible? How do we call in those with political and social power (e.g., celebrities, billionaires, politicians, police) who refuse to acknowledge their harmful actions? Thus, while Ross is correct in stating that the goal of the human rights movement is “ to end oppression ” and that call-in culture may be an effective method for achieving this long-term goal, it may also be true that a one-size-fits-all approach to achieving this goal is too narrow. Call-in culture may be less helpful where those who are marginalized and harmed are continuously silenced by their oppressors (e.g., as victims of sexual violence). In these situations, silencing perpetrators, prioritising support, and amplification of the harmed group seem more immediately important, especially if the harmed group deals with unique stereotypes and expectations based on their group identity (such as the MMM for East Asian communities). Therefore, cancel culture, as supported by the current research, may be effective in immediate harm-reduction for the harmed group in the form of feelings of collective validation and a subsequent stronger intention to work for change that may be another path to a long-term shift away from oppression.

The current research offers a novel theoretical and empirical introduction to the concept of collective validation and the understudied context of cancel culture to the existing research and theory in the social psychological literature on collective action and related topics (e.g., allyship). We found strong support that cancel culture is collectively validating for harmed groups, and that these feelings of collective validation mediate the relationship between cancel culture and collective action intentions. Therefore, we suggest and hope that future intergroup relations research on collective action and related concepts continue to utilise collective validation and cancel culture to deepen psychological understanding of collective action motivations and various psychological outcomes for harmed groups (e.g., wellbeing and life satisfaction, collective action intention and behaviour, empowerment, group identity, etc.).

Data availability statement

Ethics statement.

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Simon Fraser University Research Ethics Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

MT: writing, editing, recruiting participants, creating materials and survey, constructing measures, main data analyses, methodology and statistical analyses plan, power analyses, and theoretical background. YT: editing, recruiting participants, creating materials, focus group, and minor data analyses. SW: editing, methodology and statistical analyses plan, creation of materials, theoretical background. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

This research was funded by a CGS-M SSHRC and SPSSI Clara Mayo grant awarded to the corresponding author.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge that this research was – in part – conducted on stolen territory belonging to the Coast Salish Nations, specifically the xʷməθkʷəy̍əm (Musqueam), Sḵwx̱wú7mesh Úxwumixw (Squamish), səl̍ilw̍ətaʔɬ (Tsleil-Waututh), kʷikʷəƛ̍əm (Kwikwetlem), and q̍icə̍y̍ (Katzie) Nations, on which Simon Fraser University lies. We would also like to acknowledge that this research was funded by a CGS-M SSHRC and SPSSI Clara Mayo grant awarded to the corresponding author and that this research was conducted as a master’s thesis project which can be found on the Simon Fraser University Library website at: https://sfuprimo.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com/permalink/f/usv8m3/01SFUL_ALMA51445265030003611 .

1 Participants who spent less than 500 s on the survey had more incomplete data and tended to select the same scale values throughout (e.g., selecting five for every item). Participants who spent longer than 7,200 s on the survey were more likely to spend long periods of time on one page with questions that should only have taken minutes to complete, indicating that they may have left the survey open to complete other tasks and come back to it later.

2 See Supplementary materials for EFA.

3 No participant spent more than 7,200 s.

4 osf.io/qftn8

5 See Supplementary materials for EFA.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1181872/full#supplementary-material

  • Aguirre A. , Jr., Lio S. (2008). Spaces of mobilization: the Asian American/Pacific islander struggle for social justice . Soc. Justice 35 , 1–17. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Banet-Weiser S. (2018). Empowered: popular feminism and popular misogyny Duke University Press Durham. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barbato L. (2014). This frat is in trouble . Bustle News (Online). https://www.bustle.com/articles/41390-texas-tech-frat-under-investigation-for-disgusting-yes-means-anal-banner
  • Baron R. M., Kenny D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51 , 1173–1182. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173, PMID: [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baylon J., Cecco L. (2021). Attacks make Vancouver ‘anti-Asian hate crime capita of North America’ . The Guardian (online). https://www.theguardian.com/world/2021/may/23/vancoucer-anti-asian-hate-crimes-increase
  • Becker J. C., Wright S. C. (2011). Yet another dark side of chivalry: benevolent sexism undermines and hostile sexism motivates collective action for social change . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101 , 62–77. doi: 10.1037/a0022615, PMID: [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Becker J. C., Wright S. C. (2022). Can cross-group contact turn advantaged group members into allies? The role of inequality-delegitimizing contact and interpersonal connection . Group Process. Intergr.Relat. 25 , 1496–1515. doi: 10.1177/13684302211015273 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Becker J. C., Wright S. C., Siem B. (2022). Can cross-group contact predict advantaged group member’s willingness to engage in costly solidarity-based action? Yes, if the contact is politicized . Test. Psychom. Methodol. Appl. Psychol. 29 , 123–139. doi: 10.4473/TPM29.1.9 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Beecham M.. (2021). We don’t want groupthink and we don’t want anti- intellectualism [post]. Instagram .
  • Bradbury-Jones C., Sambrook S., Irvine F. (2011). Empowerment and being valued: a phenomenological study of nursing students' experiences of clinical practice . Nurse Educ. Today 31 , 368–372. doi: 10.1016/j.nedt.2010.07.008, PMID: [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brockell G. (2021). The long, ugly history of anti-Asian racism and violence in the US . The Washington Post (Online). https://www.washingtonpost.com/history/2021/03/18/history-anti-asian-violence-racism/
  • Carter R. T. (2007). Racism and psychological and emotional injury: recognizing and assessing race-based traumatic stress . Couns. Psychol. 35 , 13–105. doi: 10.1177/0011000006292033 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chen J. A., Zhang E., Liu C. H. (2020). Potential impact of COVID-19–related racial discrimination on the health of Asian Americans . Am. J. Public Health 110 , 1624–1627. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2020.305858, PMID: [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Droogendyk L., Louis W. R., Wright S. C. (2016). Renewed promise for positive cross-group contact: the role of supportive contact in empowering collective action . Can. J. Behav. Sci. 48 , 317–327. doi: 10.1037/cbs0000058 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Drury J. (2002). When the mobs are looking for witches to burn, nobody’s safe’: talking about the reactionary crowd . Discourse Soc. 13 , 41–73. doi: 10.1177/0957926502013001003 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Drury J., Cocking C., Beale J., Hanson C., Rapley F. (2005). The phenomenology of empowerment in collective action . Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 44 , 309–328. doi: 10.1348/014466604X18523 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Drury J., Reicher S. (1999). The intergroup dynamics of collective empowerment: substantiating the social identity model of crowd behavior . Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 2 , 381–402. doi: 10.1177/1368430299024005 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Edmondson A. C. (2004). “ Psychological safety, trust, and learning in organizations: a group- level lens ” in Trust and distrust in organizations: dilemmas and approaches . eds. Kramer R. M., Cook K. S. (Russell: Sage Foundation New York; ), 239–272. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Farbod S., Kamal M., Maulan S. (2017). Safety perception and concerns in naturalistic landscapes of urban parks in Malaysia . Secur. J. 30 , 106–122. doi: 10.1057/sj.2014.15 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Foster M. D., Tassone A., Matheson K. (2021). Tweeting about sexism motivates further activism: a social identity perspective . Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 60 , 741–764. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12431, PMID: [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Government of Canada (2021). Significant events in history of Canadians of Asian heritage: Canadians of east Asian heritage . https://www.canada.ca/en/canadian-heritage/campaigns/asian-heritage-month/important-events.html
  • Hagi S. (2019). Cancel culture is not real—At least not in the way people think TIME (online) . https://time.com/5735403/cancel-culture-is-not-real/
  • Hartling L. M., Luchetta T. (1999). Humiliation: assessing the impact of derision, degradation, and debasement . J. Prim. Prev. 19 , 259–278. doi: 10.1023/A:1022622422521 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hayes A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium . Commun. Monogr. 76 , 408–420. doi: 10.1080/03637750903310360 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hayes A. F. (2022). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach . 3rd Edn. New York.: The Guilford Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hong P. Y., Lishner D. A. (2016). General invalidation and trauma-specific invalidation as predictors of personality and subclinical psychopathology . Personal. Individ. Differ. 89 , 211–216. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.10.016 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hornsey M. J., Wohl M. J. (2013). We are sorry: intergroup apologies and their tenuous link with intergroup forgiveness . Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 24 , 1–31. doi: 10.1080/10463283.2013.822206 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kalkhoff W. (2005). Collective validation in multi-actor task groups: the effects of status differentiation . Soc. Psychol. Q. 68 , 57–74. doi: 10.1177/019027250506800105 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kambhampaty A. P., Sakaguchi H. (2020). Asian Americans share experiences of racism during COVID-19 . Time Magazine (Online). https://time.com/5858649/racism-coronavirus/
  • Lardier D. T., Opara I., Garcia-Reid P., Reid R. J. (2020). The cognitive empowerment scale: multigroup confirmatory factor analysis among youth of color . Child Adolesc. Soc. Work J. 37 , 179–193. doi: 10.1007/s10560-019-00647-2, PMID: [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lee J. (2022). Confronting the invisibility of anti-Asian racism. Brookings (Online). [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mackie D. M., Devos T., Smith E. R. (2000). Intergroup emotions: explaining offensive action tendencies in an intergroup context . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79 , 602–616. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.602, PMID: [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • MacKinnon D. P., Lockwood C. M., Williams J. (2004). Confidence limits for the indirect effect: distribution of the product and resampling methods . Multivar. Behav. Res. 39 , 99–128. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3901_4, PMID: [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moya-Garófano A., Rodríguez-Bailón R., Moya M., Megías J. L. (2021). Stranger harassment (“piropo”) and women’s self-objectification: the role of anger, happiness, and empowerment . J. Interpers. Violence 36 , 2306–2326. doi: 10.1177/0886260518760258, PMID: [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mummendey A., Klink A., Mielke R., Wenzel M., Blanz M. (1999). Socio-structural characteristics of intergroup relations and identity management strategies: results from a field study in East Germany . Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 29 , 259–285. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199903/05)29:2/3<259::AID-EJSP927>3.0.CO;2-F [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ng E. (2020). No grand pronouncements here…: reflections on cancel culture and digital media participation . Telev. New Media 21 , 621–627. doi: 10.1177/1527476420918828 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Okimoto T. G., Wenzel M. (2008). “ The symbolic meaning of transgressions: towards a unifying framework of justice restoration ” in Justice . eds. Hegtvedt K. A., Clay-Warner J. (Emerald Publishing Limited Bingley, England; ), 291–326. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Özeke-Kocabaş E., Üstündağ-Budak A. M. (2017). Validation skills in counselling and psychotherapy . Int. J. Sci. Study 5 , 319–322. doi: 10.17354/ijssNov/2017/44 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rappaport J. (1987). Terms of empowerment/exemplars of prevention: toward a theory for community psychology . Am. J. Community Psychol. 15 , 121–148. doi: 10.1007/BF00919275, PMID: [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Romano A. (2020). Why we can’t stop fighting about cancel culture. Vox (online) . https://www.vox.com/culture/2019/12/30/20879720/what-is-cancel-culture-explained-history-debate
  • Ronson J. (2016). So you’ve been publicly shamed , New York. Riverhead Books. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ross L. (2021). Don’t call people out – Call them in [video] . TEDConferences. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=xw_720iQDss&ab_channel=TED
  • Sabherwal A., Ballew M. T., van Der Linden S., Gustafson A., Goldberg M. H., Maibach E. W., et al.. (2021). The Greta Thunberg effect: familiarity with Greta Thunberg predicts intentions to engage in climate activism in the United States . J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 51 , 321–333. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12737 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Servantes I. (2014). Texas Tech fraternity learns “no means yes, yes means anal” banner was a bad idea . Complex (Online). https://www.complex.com/pop-culture/a/ian-servantes/texas-tech-fraternity-in-trouble-for-rapey-banner
  • Shao K., Lin K. (2021). Why are we so shocked by recent waves of anti-Asian violence? Toronto Star (Online). https://www.thestar.com/opinion/contributors/2021/03/19/why-are-we-so-shocked-by-recent-waves-of-anti-asian-violence.html [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shepherd L., Evans C. (2020). From gaze to outrage: the role of group-based anger in mediating the relationship between sexual objectification and collective action . Sex Roles 82 , 277–292. doi: 10.1007/s11199-019-01054-8 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shrout P. E., Bolger N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations . Psychol. Methods 7 , 422–445. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.4.422 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Simon B., Klandermans B. (2001). Politicized collective identity: a social psychological analysis . Am. Psychol. 56 , 319–331. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.4.319 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Smith B. (1997). Empowerment-the challenge is now . Empower. Organ. 5 , 120–122. doi: 10.1108/14634449710180008 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stürmer S., Simon B. (2009). Pathways to collective protest: calculation, identification, or emotion? A critical analysis of the role of group-based anger in social movement participation . J. Soc. Issues 65 , 681–705. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2009.01620.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tai C. (2020). Being Chinese-Canadian in a brave new world . Toronto Star (Online). https://www.thestar.com/opinion/contributors/2020/04/24/being-chinese-canadian-in-a-brave-new-world.html
  • Tajfel H., Turner J. C. (1979). “ An integrative theory of intergroup conflict ” in The social psychology of intergroup relations . eds. Austin W. G., Worchel S. (California : Brooks/Cole; ), 33–47. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tausch N., Becker J. C., Spears R., Christ O., Saab R., Singh P., et al.. (2011). Explaining radical group behavior: developing emotion and efficacy routes to normative and nonnormative collective action . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101 , 129–148. doi: 10.1037/a0022728 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Turner J. C., Hogg M. A., Oakes P. J., Reicher S. D., Wetherell M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: a self-categorization theory , (Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell; ). [ Google Scholar ]
  • van Zomeren M. (2013). Four core social-psychological motivations to undertake collective action . Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 7 , 378–388. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12031 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Van Zomeren M., Leach C. W., Spears R. (2012). Protesters as “passionate economists” a dynamic dual pathway model of approach coping with collective disadvantage . Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 16 , 180–199. doi: 10.1177/1088868311430835 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • van Zomeren M., Postmes T., Spears R. (2008a). Toward an integrative social identity model of collective action: a quantitative research synthesis of three socio-psychological perspectives . Psychol. Bull. 134 , 504–535. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.504 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • van Zomeren M., Spears R., Fischer A. H., Leach C. W. (2004). Put your money where your mouth is! Explaining collective action tendencies through group-based anger and group efficacy . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87 , 649–664. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.649 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • van Zomeren M., Spears R., Leach C. W. (2008b). Exploring psychological mechanisms of collective action: does relevance of group identity influence how people cope with collective disadvantage? Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 47 , 353–372. doi: 10.1348/014466607X231091 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wei M., Alvarez A. N., Ku T.-Y., Russell D. W., Bonett D. G. (2010). Development and validation of a coping with discrimination scale: factor structure, reliability, and validity . J. Couns. Psychol. 57 , 328–344. doi: 10.1037/a0019969 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weis D., Schank M. J., Matheus R. (2006). The process of empowerment: a parish nurse perspective . J. Holist. Nurs. 24 , 17–24. doi: 10.1177/0898010105282522 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wright S. C. (2010). “ Collective action and social change ” in The SAGE handbook of prejudice, stereotyping and discrimination . eds. Dovidio J. F., Glick P., Hewstone M. (California: SAGE Publications Limited California; ), 577–595. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wright J. D., Schmitt M. T., Mackay C. M., Neufeld S. D. (2020). Imagining a sustainable world: measuring cognitive alternatives to the environmental status quo . J. Environ. Psychol. 72 :101523. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101523 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yoo H. C., Burrola K. S., Steger M. F. (2010). A preliminary report on a new measure: internalization of the model minority myth measure (IM-4) and its psychological correlates among Asian American college students . J. Couns. Psychol. 57 , 114–127. doi: 10.1037/a0017871 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zimmerman M. A. (2000). Empowerment theory: Psychological, organizational and community levels of analysis . in Handbook of community psychology (pp. 43-63). Boston, MA: Springer US . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zimmerman M. A., Rappaport J. (1988). Citizen participation, perceived control, and psychological empowerment . Am. J. Community Psychol. 16 , 725–750. doi: 10.1007/BF00930023 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Share full article

Advertisement

Supported by

10 Theses About Cancel Culture

What we talk about when we talk about “cancellation.”

cancel culture research

By Ross Douthat

Opinion Columnist

Cancel culture is destroying liberalism. No, cancel culture doesn’t exist. No, it has always existed; remember when Brutus and Cassius canceled Julius Caesar? No, it exists but it’s just a bunch of rich entitled celebrities complaining that people can finally talk back to them on Twitter. No, it doesn’t exist except when it’s good and the canceled deserve it. Actually, it does exist, but — well, look, I can’t explain it to you until you’ve read at least four open letters on the subject.

These are just a few of the answers that you’ll get to a simple question — “What is this cancel culture thing, anyway?” — if you’re foolish enough to toss it, like chum, into the seething waters of the internet. They’re contradictory because the phenomenon is complicated — but not complicated enough to deter me from making 10 sweeping claims about the subject.

So here goes:

1. Cancellation, properly understood, refers to an attack on someone’s employment and reputation by a determined collective of critics, based on an opinion or an action that is alleged to be disgraceful and disqualifying.

“Reputation” and “employment” are key terms here. You are not being canceled if you are merely being heckled or insulted — if somebody describes you as a moron or a fascist or some profane alternative to “Douthat” on the internet — no matter how vivid and threatening the heckling becomes. You are decidedly at risk of cancellation, however, if your critics are calling for you to be de-platformed or fired or put out of business , and especially if the call is coming from inside the house — from within your professional community, from co-workers or employees or potential customers or colleagues, on a professional message board or Slack or some interest-specific slice of social media.

2. All cultures cancel; the question is for what, how widely and through what means.

There is no human society where you can say or do anything you like and expect to keep your reputation and your job. Reputational cancellation hung over the heads of Edith Wharton’s heroines; professional cancellation shadowed 20th-century figures like Lenny Bruce. Today, almost all critics of cancel culture have some line they draw, some figure — usually a racist or anti-Semite — that they would cancel, too. And social conservatives who criticize cancel culture, especially, have to acknowledge that we’re partly just disagreeing with today’s list of cancellation-worthy sins.

3. Cancellation isn’t exactly about free speech, but a liberal society should theoretically cancel less frequently than its rivals.

The canceled individual hasn’t lost any First Amendment rights, because there is no constitutional right to a particular job or reputation. At the same time, under its own self-understanding, liberalism is supposed to clear a wider space for debate than other political systems and allow a wider range of personal expression. So you would expect a liberal society to be slower to cancel, more inclined to separate the personal and the professional (or the ideological and the artistic), and quicker to offer opportunities to regain one’s reputation and start one’s professional life anew.

“It’s a free country,” runs the American boast, and even if it doesn’t violate the Constitution, cancellation cuts against that promise — which is one reason arguments about cancel culture so often become arguments about liberalism itself.

We are having trouble retrieving the article content.

Please enable JavaScript in your browser settings.

Thank you for your patience while we verify access. If you are in Reader mode please exit and  log into  your Times account, or  subscribe  for all of The Times.

Thank you for your patience while we verify access.

Already a subscriber?  Log in .

Want all of The Times?  Subscribe .

cancel culture research

Cancel Culture

A Critical Analysis

  • © 2022

School of Media Arts and Studies, Ohio University, Athens, USA

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

  • Provides an analytical framework for theorizing cancel culture and related phenomena in digital and non-digital spaces
  • Avoids assigning cancel culture to any particular political persuasion, or assessment on the basis of political position
  • Discusses cancel culture as a phenomenon arising at a particular juncture of cultural and political developments

41k Accesses

24 Citations

192 Altmetric

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this book

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Other ways to access

Licence this eBook for your library

Institutional subscriptions

About this book

“Cancel culture” has become one of the most charged concepts in contemporary culture and politics, but mainstream critiques from both the left and the right provide only snapshots of responses to the phenomenon. Takinga media and cultural studies perspective, this book traces the origins of cancel practices and discourses, and discusses their subsequent evolution within celebrity and fan cultures, consumer culture, and national politics in the U.S. and China. Moving beyond popular press accounts about the latest targets of cancelling or familiar free speech debates, this analysis identifies multiple lineages for both cancelling and criticisms about cancelling, underscoring the various configurations of power associated with “cancel culture” in particular cultural and political contexts. 

Similar content being viewed by others

cancel culture research

Re-membering, (Re-) appropriation, and Polyphony: SBS Independent and White Australian Memory

Deep culture in action: resignification, synecdoche, and metanarrative in the moral panic of the salem witch trials.

cancel culture research

Cultural Narratives, Early Occupy Movement, and the TEA Party: Revolts Against E-SCAD and SCAD

  • Cancel culture
  • Entertainment fandom
  • Social justice
  • Celebrity culture
  • Media activism
  • Fan studies

Table of contents (6 chapters)

Front matter, introduction, cancel culture, popular media, and fandom, cancel culture, black cultural practice, and digital activism, cancel culture, u.s. conservatism, and nation, cancel culture and digital nationalism in mainland china, back matter, authors and affiliations, about the author.

Eve Ng is Associate Professor in the School of Media Arts and Studies and the Women’s, Gender, and Sexuality Studies program at Ohio University, USA. Her interdisciplinary scholarship examines LGBTQ media, digital media cultures, and constructions of national identity. She has published in numerous journals,including Communication , Culture & Critique , Development and Change ,  Feminist Media Studies , Feminist Studies ,  International Journal of Communication , Journal of Film and Video ,  Popular Communication , and Transformative Works and Culture . 

Bibliographic Information

Book Title : Cancel Culture

Book Subtitle : A Critical Analysis

Authors : Eve Ng

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-97374-2

Publisher : Palgrave Macmillan Cham

eBook Packages : Literature, Cultural and Media Studies , Literature, Cultural and Media Studies (R0)

Copyright Information : The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022

Hardcover ISBN : 978-3-030-97373-5 Published: 24 March 2022

Softcover ISBN : 978-3-030-97376-6 Published: 25 March 2023

eBook ISBN : 978-3-030-97374-2 Published: 23 March 2022

Edition Number : 1

Number of Pages : IX, 153

Topics : Cultural Studies , Media and Communication

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

Cancel Culture: Myth or Reality?

In this section.

  • Faculty Publications
  • Publications by Centers & Initiatives
  • Student Publications

HKS Authors

See citation below for complete author information.

Pippa Norris Photo

  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Sweepstakes
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2024 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

The Mental Health Effects of Cancel Culture

Wildpixel / Getty Images

Reasons for Canceling

Cancel culture examples.

  • Positive Impacts
  • Mental Health Effects
  • Protect Yourself

Can You Avoid Being Canceled?

What is cancel culture.

Cancel culture is a form of boycott. It is the removal or "canceling" of a person, organization, product, brand, or anything else due to an issue that a community or group disapproves of or finds offensive.

One definition of cancel culture is "the popular practice of withdrawing support for...public figures and companies after they have done or said something considered objectionable or offensive." This canceling is often "performed on social media in the form of group shaming .”

In short, to be canceled means that a person or group decides to stop supporting someone or something based on a transgression that is either actual or perceived. 

Call-Out Culture vs. Cancel Culture

These terms are often used interchangeably, but there is a difference. Call-out culture is about calling attention to someone's wrongdoing and giving them a chance to learn from and correct the issue. Cancel culture does not give this opportunity and, instead, immediately labels them as bad .

Origin of Cancel Culture

Although canceling is often used in response to sexist behavior , the term itself appears to originate from sexist humor. One of the earliest references came from the 1991 film New Jack City. Nino Brown (played by Wesley Snipes), referring to a girlfriend, mercilessly states, “Cancel that [expletive]. I’ll buy another one.”

The term gained traction in 2014 thanks to an episode of VH1's reality show Love and Hip-Hop: New York . In this episode, music executive and record producer Cisco Rosado ended an argument with his girlfriend by saying, "You're canceled.”

In earlier days, the word "cancel" was often used on social media as a way for a person who was part of the Black LGBTQ+ community to show disapproval of another person's actions. It wasn’t until later that canceling someone involved boycotting them professionally.

A 2020 survey conducted by Pew Research Center highlights the controversy surrounding cancel culture. While 38% of people say that calling someone out on social media punishes those who don't deserve it, 58% feel that it helps hold people accountable for their actions.

Some of the top reasons cited for canceling include:

  • To serve as a teaching moment
  • To get the person to consider the consequences of their statements
  • To expose racism or sexism
  • To get people to think before they speak
  • To hold someone accountable for their statements or behaviors

Some examples of cancel culture include:

  • J.K. Rowling , the author of Harry Potter , was canceled after making a comment on Twitter in June of 2020 that offended some members of the transgender community . The tweet in question was retweeted over 95,000 times and drew more than 46,000 comments.
  • Mike Lindell , the CEO of MyPillow, was canceled in 2021 after supporting the claims of voter fraud against former President Donald Trump. At least two banks joined in on the canceling in 2022 after phone call recordings were subpoenaed by the January 6 House select committee, ultimately telling Lindell to "leave their bank" as he was a "reputation risk."
  • Chrissy Teigen , a model and bestselling cookbook author, was also canceled in 2021 after several people exposed her for sending " mean tweets ," ultimately causing Teigen to step away from her cleaning supplies company and be replaced by Netflix in the second season of "Never Have I Ever."

Positive Impact of Cancel Culture

Cancel culture can help combat wrongdoings and address inequalities . In 2016, for example, many members of the film community boycotted the Oscars because of the lack of diversity among nominees. This helped promote social change and, in 2019, the Oscars set a record for the most nominations for Black directors ever.

A community that unites for a common cause can be empowering. It can also make people think twice before behaving inappropriately or posting potentially offensive thoughts and opinions.

Mental Health Effects of Cancel Culture

There are also negative effects of cancel culture, some of which are related to mental health. The impact of cancel culture on mental health depends on whether you are the one being canceled, the canceler, or a bystander.

Effects on the Canceled

Unfortunately, canceling often turns into bullying . Like bullying, if you've been canceled, you may feel ostracized, socially isolated, and lonely. And research shows that loneliness is associated with higher anxiety, depression, and suicide rates.  

If you are canceled, it can also feel as if everyone is giving up on you before you've even had the chance to apologize (let alone change your behavior). Instead of creating a dialogue to help you understand how your actions hurt others, the cancelers shut off all communication, essentially robbing you of the opportunity to learn and grow from your mistakes . 

To grow and become a better person, it's important to realize you've made a mistake, attempt to fix that mistake, and then take the proper steps to ensure that you don't make the same mistake again .

Effects on the Canceler

You have the right to set your own boundaries and to decide what uplifts and what offends you. You also have the right to decide to whom and what you give your attention, money, and support.

But canceling the offending person (or brand) doesn't always cause them to change their beliefs or lead to lasting change. It can even make them dig in their heels in an effort to defend their ego and reputation.

In some cases, canceling has the opposite effect of what was desired. One example is the docuseries "Surviving R. Kelly ." While this TV series prompted many to push for a sex crimes conviction against the musician, it also created a 126% increase in on-demand streams of Kelly's music the day after the premiere.

Effects on the Bystander

Cancel culture doesn’t just affect the canceled and the cancelers. It can also wreak havoc on onlookers’ mental health.

After seeing so many people being canceled, some bystanders are plagued with fear. They become overwhelmed with anxiety that people will turn on them if they fully express themselves. This can cause them to keep their thoughts bottled up instead of talking about and working through their opinions and emotions.

Bystanders might also worry that others will find something in their pasts to use against them. Or they may fear that every word they say or write is going to be examined under a microscope and construed as offensive, even if it wasn't meant to be.

So, instead of saying how they feel about an event or situation, bystanders may choose to remain silent. This can lead them to be weighed down with guilt long after the event or situation has passed—guilt for not standing up for someone else when they had the chance.

The idea that cancel culture has caused some people to fall silent or not feel comfortable sharing what is on their minds has caused some to debate whether it presents an issue with the right to free speech.

How to Protect Your Mental Health

Though you can't control how others behave, you can control your own behavior (as well as how you respond to negativity). Here are some actions you can take to help protect your mental health with regard to cancel culture:

  • Avoid posting when your emotions are heightened . Try not to post when you're feeling overly emotional. If someone says or does something that pushes your buttons, don't rush to your keyboard. Instead, take a few deep breaths and give yourself time to calm down . While you may forget what you said or wrote a day, week, or month from now, the internet never forgets.
  • Have others review your post first . Sometimes it's hard to recognize when our own words may come across as offensive or aggressive. Having someone else review your posts first can help bring any potential issues to your attention. This gives you the opportunity to avoid sharing thoughts or opinions that could inadvertently hurt someone else.
  • If you were wrong, apologize . If you were canceled for saying or doing something and now feel bad about it, apologize. But before you do, give yourself time to craft a genuine, well-thought-out response (this also gives time for the attention to die down). Then, when you're ready, share your apology. It might not be accepted by everyone. But if it's from the heart, some people will recognize this and allow it to soften their views of you—or at least reduce their desire to make you their top public enemy.
  • Try to see the other side . If someone speaks out against you, your first reaction may be to dig in and stand your ground. However, you may get further by trying to truly understand how your words or actions may have hurt or offended someone else. Gaining this understanding can bridge the communication gap. It can also help keep you from making the same mistake again.
  • Spend less time online . It's okay to take a break from social media . Unplugging every now and then may help improve your mental health. One study found that cutting back on social media use decreases loneliness and depression .
  • Talk to someone . If you're experiencing cancel culture firsthand and are not sure how to recover, consider reaching out to someone you trust, such as a family member or close friend. If you're not comfortable talking to someone you know, you can also seek professional help . Having someone to confide in can make a huge difference in the way you feel.

Consider that everyone has different backgrounds, experiences, and beliefs. Although yours have caused you to view the world one way, not everyone has that same view. Being aware of this (and open-minded ) can help keep you from saying or doing something that makes you a target.

It's also helpful to remember that you don't have to intend to be offensive in order to offend. Perception becomes reality, so if someone perceives your words or actions as offensive, it doesn't matter what you intended. The damage is done.

So, instead of trying to convince others that you didn't mean to offend them, recognize that they are offended and work to find ways to move forward. Learn from the experience and use it to help you become a stronger, more caring, and empathetic person.

A Word From Verywell

Some aspects of cancel culture can be useful in holding people and organizations accountable for bad behavior. On the flip side, it can take bullying to a new level, damaging the mental well-being of everyone involved.

The key to overcoming any sort of ostracism or rejection is to not allow the things that are said or done to define who you are as a person. And don't be afraid to reach out for help. Having someone in your corner can help you feel more connected and less alone.

Dictionary.com. Cancel culture: What does cancel culture mean?

Romano A. Why we can’t stop fighting about cancel culture . Vox .

Clark MD. DRAG THEM: A brief etymology of so-called “cancel culture" . Communic Public . 2020;5(3-4):88-92. doi:10.1177/2057047320961562

Vogels E, Anderson M, Porteus M, et al. Americans and 'cancel culture': Where some see calls for accountability, others see censorship, punishment . Pew Research Center.

Camero K. What is 'cancel culture'? J.K. Rowling controversy leaves writers, scholars debating . Miami Herald .

Teh C, Lahut J. MyPillow CEO Mike Lindell says he's 'disgusted' with 2 banks he claims are cutting ties with him over 'cancel culture' after his phone records were subpoenaed by the January 6 committee . Yahoo! News .

Ali R. 'There is no winning': Chrissy Teigen opens up about being in the 'cancel club' . USA Today .

Collins KA. The 2019 Oscar nominations are a long-overdue net win for black filmmakers . Vanity Fair .

Beutel ME, Klein EM, Brähler E, et al. Loneliness in the general population: prevalence, determinants and relations to mental health . BMC Psychiatry . 2017;17(1):97. doi:10.1186/s12888-017-1262-x

Dudenhoefer N. Is cancel culture effective? . University of Central Florida.

University of Pennsylvania. Free speech advocate discusses growing talk of 'cancel culture' . Penn Today .

Hunt MG, Marx R, Lipson C, Young J. No more FOMO: Limiting social media decreases loneliness and depression . J Soc Clin Psychol . 2018;37(10):751-768. doi:10.1521/jscp.2018.37.10.751

By Lindsey Toler Lindsey Toler, MPH, is a public health professional with over a decade of experience writing and editing health and science communications. 

The second wave of “cancel culture”

How the concept has evolved to mean different things to different people.

by Aja Romano

An illustration of a laptop computer with a hand and a courtroom gavel coming out of its screen.

“Cancel culture,” as a concept, feels inescapable. The phrase is all over the news, tossed around in casual social media conversation; it’s been linked to everything from free speech debates to Mr. Potato Head .

It sometimes seems all-encompassing, as if all forms of contemporary discourse must now lead, exhaustingly and endlessly, either to an attempt to “cancel” anyone whose opinions cause controversy or to accusations of cancel culture in action, however unwarranted.

In the rhetorical furor, a new phenomenon has emerged: the weaponization of cancel culture by the right.

Across the US, conservative politicians have launched legislation seeking to do the very thing they seem to be afraid of: Cancel supposedly left-wing businesses, organizations, and institutions; see, for example, national GOP figures threatening to punish Major League Baseball for standing against a Georgia voting restrictions law by removing MLB’s federal antitrust exemption.

Meanwhile, Fox News has stoked outrage and alarmism over cancel culture, including trying to incite Gen X to take action against the nebulous problem. Tucker Carlson, one of the network’s most prominent personalities, has emphatically embraced the anti-cancel culture discourse, claiming liberals are trying to cancel everything from Space Jam to the Fourth of July .

The idea of canceling began as a tool for marginalized communities to assert their values against public figures who retained power and authority even after committing wrongdoing — but in its current form, we see how warped and imbalanced the power dynamics of the conversation really are.

All along, debate about cancel culture has obscured its roots in a quest to attain some form of meaningful accountability for public figures who are typically answerable to no one. But after centuries of ideological debate turning over questions of free speech, censorship, and, in recent decades, “political correctness,” it was perhaps inevitable that the mainstreaming of cancel culture would obscure the original concerns that canceling was meant to address. Now it’s yet another hyperbolic phase of the larger culture war.

The core concern of cancel culture — accountability — remains as crucial a topic as ever. But increasingly, the cancel culture debate has become about how we communicate within a binary, right versus wrong framework. And a central question is not whether we can hold one another accountable, but how we can ever forgive.

Cancel culture has evolved rapidly to mean very different things to different people

It’s only been about six years since the concept of “cancel culture” began trickling into the mainstream. The phrase has long circulated within Black culture, perhaps paying homage to Nile Rodgers’s 1981 single “Your Love Is Cancelled.” As I wrote in my earlier explainer on the origins of cancel culture , the concept of canceling a whole person originated in the 1991 film New Jack City and percolated for years before finally emerging online among Black Twitter in 2014 thanks to an episode of Love and Hip-Hop: New York. Since then, the term has undergone massive shifts in meaning and function.

Early on, it most frequently popped up on social media, as people attempted to collectively “cancel,” or boycott, celebrities they found problematic. As a term with roots in Black culture, it has some resonance with Black empowerment movements, as far back as the civil rights boycotts of the 1950s and ’60s . This original usage also promotes the idea that Black people should be empowered to reject cultural figures or works that spread harmful ideas. As Anne Charity Hudley, the chair of linguistics of African America at the University of California Santa Barbara, told me in 2019 , “When you see people canceling Kanye, canceling other people, it’s a collective way of saying, ‘We elevated your social status, your economic prowess, [and] we’re not going to pay attention to you in the way that we once did. ... ‘I may have no power, but the power I have is to [ignore] you.’”

As the logic behind wanting to “cancel” specific messages and behaviors caught on, many members of the public, as well as the media, conflated it with adjacent trends involving public shaming, callouts, and other forms of public backlash . (The media sometimes refers to all of these ideas collectively as “ outrage culture .”) But while cancel culture overlaps and aligns with many related ideas, it’s also always been inextricably linked to calls for accountability.

As a concept, cancel culture entered the mainstream alongside hashtag-oriented social justice movements like #BlackLivesMatter and #MeToo — giant social waves that were effective in shifting longstanding narratives about victims and criminals, and in bringing about actual prosecutions in cases like those of Bill Cosby and Harvey Weinstein . It is also frequently used interchangeably with “woke” political rhetoric , an idea that is itself tied to the 2014 rise of the Black Lives Matter protests. In similar ways, both “wokeness” and “canceling” are tied to collectivized demands for more accountability from social systems that have long failed marginalized people and communities.

But over the past few years, many right-wing conservatives, as well as liberals who object to more strident progressive rhetoric, have developed the view that “cancel culture” is a form of harassment intended to silence anyone who sets a foot out of line under the nebulous tenets of “woke” politics . So the idea now represents a vast assortment of objectives and can hold wildly different connotations, depending on whom you’re talking to.

  • “Wokeness is a problem and we all know it”

Taken in good faith, the concept of “canceling” a person is really about questions of accountability — about how to navigate a social and public sphere in which celebrities, politicians, and other public figures who say or do bad things continue to have significant platforms and influence. In fact, actor LeVar Burton recently suggested the entire idea should be recast as “consequence culture.”

“I think it’s misnamed,” Burton told the hosts of The View . “I think we have a consequence culture. And that consequences are finally encompassing everybody in the society, whereas they haven’t been ever in this country.”

Within the realm of good faith, the larger conversation around these questions can then expand to contain nuanced considerations of what the consequences of public misbehavior should be, how and when to rehabilitate the reputation of someone who’s been “canceled,” and who gets to decide those things.

Taken in bad faith, however, “cancel culture” becomes an omniscient and dangerous specter: a woke, online social justice mob that’s ready to rise up and attack anyone, even other progressives, at the merest sign of dissent. And it’s this — the fear of a nebulous mob of cancel-happy rabble-rousers — that conservatives have used to their political advantage.

Conservatives are using fear of cancel culture as a cudgel

Critics of cancel culture typically portray whoever is doing the canceling as wielding power against innocent victims of their wrath. From 2015 on, a variety of news outlets, whether through opinion articles or general reporting , have often framed cancel culture as “ mob rule .”

In 2019, the New Republic’s Osita Nwanevu observed just how frequently some media outlets have compared cancel culture to violent political uprisings, ranging from ethnocide to torture under dictatorial regimes. Such an exaggerated framework has allowed conservative media to depict cancel culture as an urgent societal issue. Fox News pundits, for example, have made cancel culture a focal part of their coverage . In one recent survey , people who voted Republican were more than twice as likely to know what “cancel culture” was, compared with Democrats and other voters, even though in the current dominant understanding of cancel culture, Democrats are usually the ones doing the canceling.

“The conceit that the conservative right has gotten so many people to adopt , beyond divorcing the phrase from its origins in Black queer communities, is an obfuscation of the power relations of the stakeholders involved,” journalist Shamira Ibrahim told Vox in an email. “It got transformed into a moral panic akin to being able to irrevocably ruin the powerful with just the press of a keystroke, when it in actuality doesn’t wield nearly as much power as implied by the most elite.”

You wouldn’t know that to listen to right-wing lawmakers and media figures who have latched onto an apocalyptic scenario in which the person or subject who’s being criticized is in danger of being censored, left jobless, or somehow erased from history — usually because of a perceived left-wing mob.

This is a fear that the right has weaponized. At the 2020 Republican National Convention , at least 11 GOP speakers — about a third of those who took the stage during the high-profile event — addressed cancel culture as a concerning political phenomenon. President Donald Trump himself declared that “The goal of cancel culture is to make decent Americans live in fear of being fired, expelled, shamed, humiliated and driven from society as we know it.” One delegate resolution at the RNC specifically targeted cancel culture , describing a trend toward “erasing history, encouraging lawlessness, muting citizens, and violating free exchange of ideas, thoughts, and speech.”

Ibrahim pointed out that in addition to re-waging the war on political correctness that dominated the 1990s by repackaging it as a war on cancel culture, right-wing conservatives have also “attempted to launch the same rhetorical battles” across numerous fronts, attempting to rebrand the same calls for accountability and consequences as “woke brigade, digital lynch mobs, outrage culture and call-out culture.” Indeed, it’s because of the collective organizational power that online spaces provide to marginalized communities, she argued, that anti-cancel culture rhetoric focuses on demonizing them.

  • The “free speech debate” isn’t really about free speech

Social media is “one of the few spaces that exists for collective feedback and where organizing movements that threaten [conservatives’] social standing have begun,” Ibrahim said, “thus compelling them to invert it into a philosophical argument that doesn’t affect just them, but potentially has destructive effects on censorship for even the working-class individual.”

This potential has nearly become reality through recent forms of Republican-driven legislation around the country. The first wave involved overt censorship , with lawmakers pushing to ban texts like the New York Times’s 1619 Project from educational usage at publicly funded schools and universities. Such censorship could seriously curtail free speech at these institutions — an ironic example of the broader kind of censorship that is seemingly a core fear about cancel culture.

A recent wave of legislation has been directed at corporations as a form of punishment for crossing Republicans. After both Delta Air Lines and Major League Baseball spoke out against Georgia lawmakers’ passage of a restrictive voting rights bill , Republican lawmakers tried to target the companies, tying their public statements to cancel culture. State lawmakers tried and failed to pass a bill stripping Delta of a tax exemption . And some national GOP figures have threatened to punish MLB by removing its exemption from federal antitrust laws. Senate Minority Leader Mitch McConnell said that “corporations will invite serious consequences if they become a vehicle for far-left mobs.”

But for all the hysteria and the actual crackdown attempts lawmakers have enacted, even conservatives know that most of the hand-wringing over cancellation is performative. CNN’s AJ Willingham pointed out how easily anti-cancel culture zeal can break down, noting that although the 2021 Conservative Political Action Conference (CPAC) was called “America Uncanceled,” the organization wound up removing a scheduled speaker who had expressed anti-Semitic viewpoints. And Fox News fired a writer last year after he was found to have a history of making racist, homophobic, and sexist comments online.

These moves suggest that though they may decry “woke” hysteria, conservatives also sometimes want consequences for extremism and other harmful behavior — at least when the shaming might fall on them as well.

“This dissonance reveals cancel culture for what it is,” Willingham wrote. “Accountability for one’s actions.”

CPAC’s swift levying of consequences in the case of a potentially anti-Semitic speaker is revealing on a number of levels, not only because it gives away the lie beneath concerns that “cancel culture” is something profoundly new and dangerous, but also because the conference actually had the power to take action and hold the speaker accountable. Typically, the apocryphal “social justice mob” has no such ability. Actually canceling a whole person is much harder to do than opponents of cancel culture might make it sound — nearly impossible, in fact.

Very few “canceled” public figures suffer significant career setbacks

It’s true that some celebrities have effectively been canceled, in the sense that their actions have resulted in major consequences, including job losses and major reputational declines, if not a complete end to their careers.

Consider Harvey Weinstein , Bill Cosby , R. Kelly , and Kevin Spacey , who faced allegations of rape and sexual assault that became impossible to ignore, and who were charged with crimes for their offenses. They have all effectively been “canceled” — Weinstein and Cosby because they’re now convicted criminals, Kelly because he’s in prison awaiting trial , and Spacey because while all charges against him to date have been dropped, he’s too tainted to hire.

Along with Roseanne Barr, who lost her hit TV show after a racist tweet , and Louis C.K., who saw major professional setbacks after he admitted to years of sexual misconduct against female colleagues, their offenses were serious enough to irreparably damage their careers, alongside a push to lessen their cultural influence.

But usually, to effectively cancel a public figure is much more difficult. In typical cases where “cancel culture” is applied to a famous person who does something that incurs criticism, that person rarely faces serious long-term consequences. During the past year alone, a number of individuals and institutions have faced public backlash for troubling behavior or statements — and a number of them thus far have either weathered the storm or else departed their jobs or restructured their operations of their own volition.

For example, beloved talk show host Ellen DeGeneres has come under fire in recent years for a number of reasons, from palling around with George W. Bush to accusing the actress Dakota Johnson of not inviting her to a party to, most seriously, allegedly fostering an abusive and toxic workplace . The toxic workplace allegations had an undeniable impact on DeGeneres’s ratings, with The Ellen DeGeneres Show losing over 40 percent of its viewership in the 2020–’21 TV season. But DeGeneres has not literally been canceled; her daytime talk show has been confirmed for a 19th season, and she continues to host other TV series like HBO Max’s Ellen’s Next Great Designer .

Another TV host recently felt similar heat but has so far retained his job: In February, The Bachelor franchise underwent a reckoning due to a long history of racial insensitivity and lack of diversity, culminating in the announcement that longtime host Chris Harrison would be “ stepping aside for a period of time.” But while Harrison won’t be hosting the upcoming season of The Bachelorette , ABC still lists him as the franchise host, and some franchise alums have come forward to defend him . (It is unclear whether Harrison will return as a host in the future, though he has said he plans to do so and has been working with race educators and engaging in a personal accountability program of “counsel, not cancel.”)

In many cases, instead of costing someone their career, the allegation of having been “canceled” instead bolsters sympathy for the offender, summoning a host of support from both right-wing media and the public. In March 2021, concerns that Dr. Seuss was being “canceled” over a decision by the late author’s publisher to stop printing a small selection of works containing racist imagery led to a run on Seuss’s books that landed him on bestseller lists. And although J.K. Rowling sparked massive outrage and calls to boycott all things Harry Potter after she aired transphobic views in a 2020 manifesto, sales of the Harry Potter books increased tremendously in her home country of Great Britain.

A few months later, 58 British public figures including playwright Tom Stoppard signed an open letter supporting Rowling’s views and calling her the target of “an insidious, authoritarian and misogynistic trend in social media.” And in December, the New York Times not only reviewed the author’s latest title — a new children’s book called The Ickabog — but praised the story’s “moral rectitude,” with critic Sarah Lyall summing up, “It made me weep with joy.” It was an instant bestseller .

In light of these contradictions, it’s tempting to declare that the idea of “canceling” someone has already lost whatever meaning it once had. But for many detractors, the “real” impact of cancel culture isn’t about famous people anyway.

Rather, they worry, “cancel culture” and the polarizing rhetoric it enables really impacts the non-famous members of society who suffer its ostensible effects — and that, even more broadly, it may be threatening our ability to relate to each other at all.

The debate around cancel culture began as a search for accountability. It may ultimately be about encouraging empathy.

It’s not only right-wing conservatives who are wary of cancel culture. In 2019, former President Barack Obama decried cancel culture and “woke” politics, framing the phenomenon as people “be[ing] as judgmental as possible about other people” and adding, “That’s not activism.”

At a recent panel devoted to making a nonpartisan “ Case Against Cancel Culture ,” former ACLU president Nadine Strossen expressed great concern over cancel culture’s chilling effect on the non-famous. “I constantly encounter students who are so fearful of being subjected to the Twitter mob that they are engaging in self-censorship,” she said. Strossen cited as one such chilling effect the isolated instances of students whose college admissions had been rescinded on the basis of racist social media posts.

In his recent book Cancel This Book: The Progressive Case Against Cancel Culture , human rights lawyer and free speech advocate Dan Kovalik argues that cancel culture is basically a giant self-own, a product of progressive semantics that causes the left to cannibalize itself.

“Unfortunately, too many on the left, wielding the cudgel of ‘cancel culture,’ have decided that certain forms of censorship and speech and idea suppression are positive things that will advance social justice,” Kovalik writes . “I fear that those who take this view are in for a rude awakening.”

Kovalik’s worries are partly grounded in a desire to preserve free speech and condemn censorship. But they’re also grounded in empathy. As America’s ideological divide widens, our patience with opposing viewpoints seems to be waning in favor of a type of society-wide “cancel and move on” approach, even though studies suggest that approach does nothing to change hearts and minds. Kovalik points to a survey published in 2020 that found that in 700 interactions, “deep listening” — including “respectful, non-judgmental conversations” — was 102 times more effective than brief interactions in a canvassing campaign for then-presidential candidate Joe Biden.

Across the political spectrum, wariness toward the idea of “cancel culture” has increased — but outside of right-wing political spheres, that wariness isn’t so centered on the hyper-specific threat of losing one’s job or career due to public backlash. Rather, the term “cancel culture” functions as shorthand for an entire mode of polarized, aggressive social engagement.

Journalist (and Vox contributor) Zeeshan Aleem has argued that contemporary social media engenders a mode of communication he calls “disinterpretation,” in which many participants are motivated to join the conversation not because they want to promote communication, or even to engage with the original opinion, but because they seek to intentionally distort the discourse.

In this type of interaction, as Aleem observed in a recent Substack post, “Commentators are constantly being characterized as believing things they don’t believe, and entire intellectual positions are stigmatized based on vague associations with ideas that they don’t have any substantive affiliation with.” The goal of such willful misinterpretation, he argued, is conformity — to be seen as aligned with the “correct” ideological standpoint in a world where stepping out of alignment results in swift backlash, ridicule, and cancellation.

Such an antagonistic approach “effectively treats public debate as a battlefield,” he wrote. He continued:

It’s illustrative of a climate in which nothing is untouched by polarization, in which everything is a proxy for some broader orientation which must be sorted into the bin of good/bad, socially aware/problematic, savvy/out of touch, my team/the enemy. ... We’re tilting toward a universe in which all discourse is subordinate to activism; everything is a narrative, and if you don’t stay on message then you’re contributing to the other team on any given issue. What this does is eliminate the possibility of public ambiguity, ambivalence, idiosyncrasy, self-interrogation.

The problem with this style of communication is that in a world where every argument gets flattened into a binary under which every opinion and every person who publicly shares their thoughts must be either praised or canceled, few people are morally righteous enough to challenge that binary without their own motives and biases then being called into question. The question becomes, as Aleem reframed it for me: “How does someone avoid the reality that their claims of being disinterpreted will be disinterpreted?”

“When people demand good-faith engagement, it can often be dismissed as a distraction tactic or whining about being called out,” he explained, noting that some responses to his original Twitter thread on the subject assumed he must be complaining about just such a callout.

Other complications can arise, such as when the people who are protesting against this type of bad-faith discourse are also criticized for problematic statements or behavior , or perceived as having too much privilege to wholly understand the situation. Remember, the origins of cancel culture are rooted in giving marginalized members of society the ability to seek accountability and change, especially from people who hold a disproportionate amount of wealth, power, and privilege.

“[W]hat people do when they invoke dog whistles like ‘cancel culture’ and ‘culture wars,’” Danielle Butler wrote for the Root in 2018, “is illustrate their discomfort with the kinds of people who now have a voice and their audacity to direct it towards figures with more visibility and power.”

But far too often, people who call for accountability on social media seem to slide quickly into wanting to administer punishment instead. In some cases, this process really does play out with a mob mentality, one that seems bent on inflicting pain and hurt while allowing no room for growth and change, showing no mercy, and offering no real forgiveness — let alone allowing for the possibility that the mob itself might be entirely unjustified.

See, for example, trans writer Isabel Fall, who wrote a short story in 2020 that angered many readers with its depiction of gender dysphoria through the lens of militaristic warfare. (The story has since become a finalist for a Hugo Award.) Because Fall published under a pseudonym, people who disliked the story assumed she must be transphobic rather than a trans woman wrestling with her own dysphoria. Fall was harassed, doxed, forcibly outed, and driven offline . These types of “cancellations” can happen without consideration for the person being canceled, even when that person apologizes — or, as in Fall’s case, even when they had little if anything to be sorry about.

The conflation of antagonized social media debates with the more serious aims to make powerful people face consequences is part of the problem. “I think the messy and turbulent evolution of speech norms online influences people’s perception of what’s called cancel culture,” Aleem said. He added that he’s grown “resistant to using the term [cancel culture] because it’s become so hard to pin down.”

“People connect boycotts with de-platforming speakers on college campuses,” he observed, “with social media harassment, with people being fired abruptly for breaching a taboo in a viral video.” The result is an environment where social media is a double-edged sword: “One could argue,” Aleem said, “that there’s now public input on issues [that wasn’t available] before, and that’s good for civil society, but that the vehicle through which that input comes produces some civically unhealthy ways of expression.”

Prevailing confusion about cancel culture hasn’t stopped it from becoming culturally and politically entrenched

If the conversation around cancel culture is unhealthy, then one can argue that the social systems cancel culture is trying to target are even more unhealthy — and that, for many people, is the bottom line.

The concept of canceling someone was created by communities of people who’ve never had much power to begin with. When people in those communities attempt to demand accountability by canceling someone, the odds are still stacked against them. They’re still the ones without the social, political, or professional power to compel someone into meaningful atonement, but they can at least be vocal by calling for a collective boycott.

The push by right-wing lawmakers and pundits to use the concept as a tool to vilify the left, liberals, and the powerless upends the original logic of cancel culture, Ibrahim told me. “It is being used to obscure marginalized voices by inverting the victim and the offender, and disingenuously affording disproportionate impact to the reach of a single voice — which has historically long been silenced — to now being the silencer of cis, male, and wealthy individuals,” she said.

And that approach is both expanding and growing more visible. What’s more, it is a divide not just between ideologies, but also between tactical approaches in navigating those ideological differences and dealing with wrongdoing.

“It effectuates a slippery-slope argument by taking a rhetorical scenario and pushing it to really absurdist levels, and furthermore asking people to suspend their implicit understanding of social constructs of power and class,” Ibrahim said. “It mutates into, ‘If I get canceled, then anyone can get canceled.’” She pointed out that usually, the supposedly “canceled” individual suffers no real long-term harm — “particularly when you give additional time for a person to regroup from a scandal. The media cycle iterates quicker than ever in present day.”

She suggested that perhaps the best approach to combating the escalation of cancel culture hysteria into a political weapon is to refuse to let those with power shape the way the conversation plays out.

“I think our remit, if anything, is to challenge that reframing and ask people to define the stakes of what material quality of life and liberty was actually lost,” she said.

In other words, the way cancel culture is discussed in the media might make it seem like something to fear and avoid at all costs, an apocalyptic event that will destroy countless lives and livelihoods, but in most cases, it’s probably not. That’s not to suggest that no one will ever be held accountable, or that powerful people won’t continue to be asked to answer for their transgressions. But the greater worry is still that people with too much power might use it for bad ends.

At its best, cancel culture has been about rectifying power imbalances and redistributing power to those who have little of it. Instead, it now seems that the concept may have become a weapon for people in power to use against those it was intended to help.

Most Popular

  • Michelle Obama articulated something Democrats have been afraid to say
  • Republicans ask the Supreme Court to disenfranchise thousands of swing state voters
  • The major political transformation flying under the radar at the DNC
  • What Barack Obama’s DNC speech was actually about
  • The case of the nearly 7,000 missing pancreases

Today, Explained

Understand the world with a daily explainer plus the most compelling stories of the day.

 alt=

This is the title for the native ad

 alt=

More in Explainers

Why Indian doctors are protesting after the rape and death of a colleague

India has systemic problems with sexual violence.

How Raygun earned her spot — fair and square — as an Olympics breaker

The truth behind the ongoing controversy over the highly memeable dancer.

What we know about Trump’s claim that Iran hacked his campaign

The FBI is now investigating the former president’s claim of Iranian interference with his campaign.

How the UK’s far right used a local tragedy to spur chaos

Rumors and disinformation have fueled violence across the country.

The attack on US troops in Iraq, explained

What the attack on US troops in Iraq says about a wider Middle East war.

Bangladesh’s prime minister just fled the country in a helicopter. Why?

Bangladesh’s Sheikh Hasina is out. What comes next?

  • For Parents
  • For Educators
  • Our Work and Impact
  • About Digital Citizenship
  • Digital Citizenship Curriculum
  • Digital Citizenship (U.K.)
  • Lesson Collections
  • All Lesson Plans
  • Digital Life Dilemmas
  • SEL in Digital Life Resource Center
  • Implementation Guide
  • Toolkits by Topic
  • Digital Citizenship Week
  • Digital Connections (Grades 6–8)
  • Digital Compass™ (Grades 6–8)
  • Digital Passport™ (Grades 3–5)
  • Social Media TestDrive (Grades 6–8)

cancel culture research

AI Literacy for Grades 6–12

  • All Apps and Websites
  • Curated Lists
  • Best in Class
  • Common Sense Selections
  • About the Privacy Program
  • Privacy Evaluations
  • Privacy Articles
  • Privacy Direct (Free download)
  • Free Back-to-School Templates
  • 21 Activities to Start School
  • AI Movies, Podcasts, & Books
  • Learning Podcasts
  • Books for Digital Citizenship
  • ChatGPT and Beyond
  • Should Your School Have Cell Phone Ban?
  • Digital Well-Being Discussions
  • Supporting LGBTQ+ Students
  • Offline Digital Citizenship
  • Teaching with Tech
  • Movies in the Classroom
  • Social & Emotional Learning
  • Digital Citizenship
  • Tech & Learning
  • News and Media Literacy
  • Common Sense Recognized Educators
  • Common Sense Education Ambassadors
  • Browse Events and Training
  • AI Foundations for Educators
  • Digital Citizenship Teacher Training
  • Modeling Digital Habits Teacher Training
  • Student Privacy Teacher Training

cancel culture research

Training Course: AI Foundations for Educators

cancel culture research

Earn your Common Sense Education badge today!

  • Family Engagement Toolkit
  • Digital Citizenship Resources for Families

Family Tech Planners

Family and community engagement program.

  • Workshops for Families with Kids Age 0–8
  • Workshops for Middle and High School Families
  • Kids and Tech Video Series

cancel culture research

  • Get Our Newsletter

A photo of a teacher helping a student learn using a laptop computer

Back-to-School Hub

Free digital citizenship resources for back-to-school, from AI to cellphone bans and beyond!

What Is Cancel Culture, and Does It Change Things for the Better?

Topics:   Digital Citizenship Social & Emotional Learning

Daniel Vargas Campos

Use this free lesson plan to help students think critically about resolving conflicts and seeking social change in the digital world.

Four students engaging in a class discussion

Grades: 8-12

Subjects: Digital Citizenship, SEL, Media Literacy, Social Studies

What is cancel culture? A 2021 report from Pew Research Center found that the American public is deeply divided over how to define it. Some people view it as a way to hold a person or group of people accountable for a past misdeed. Others see it as an unfair judgment of someone's character, or even the practice of censoring people whose opinions we don't agree with.

One of the common threads among these varying definitions of "cancel culture" is the idea that it involves someone or something being publicly called out online. In this lesson, you can help students consider where they stand. Is "canceling" someone ever a good way to get a just outcome? Could it sometimes go too far?

Use these lesson activities to help your students think deeply about the impact of cancel culture -- both among peers and directed at people or institutions in power -- and explore positive strategies for resolving conflict and seeking accountability.

Learning objectives:

  • Define cancel culture and identify the benefits, drawbacks, and impacts of calling people out online.
  • Practice considering all dimensions of a conflict in order to determine the most effective tool or approach for addressing it.
  • Build support of others through positive strategies for conflict resolution, such as "calling in." 

This lesson deep dive has three independent parts. You can string them together, or pull them apart to complement other parts of your curriculum.

Doodle of an analog clock

Quick Activity: Does Canceling Someone Help Them Change? (25 minutes)

Doodle of the silhouette of two faces looking at each other

Dilemma Discussion: Accidental Activist (45 minutes)

Doodle of a megaphone

Media Creation: "Call In" and Build Support (time varies)

Note: All of these lessons are free, but you'll need to sign in (or create an account) to access the printable handouts.

Does Canceling Someone Help Them Change? 

Prep for teachers:

  • Estimated activity time: 25 minutes
  • Preview the video " Does Canceling Someone Help Them Change? "
  • Read over the teacher version of the "Call-Out Culture" handout , where you'll find facilitation guidance and the discussion questions.
  • Make copies of the  "Call-Out Culture" student handout (or access the Google Docs version from the handout).

Activity steps:

1. Before showing the video, ask: What does it mean to "cancel" someone? What does it look like to "cancel" someone?

Explain that cancel culture is a complex topic that requires us to think critically about who is causing the harm, what their motive is, the degree of harm, and several other factors. This video and discussion focuses on peer-to-peer canceling.

2. Show the video  Does Canceling Someone Help Them Change?  and have students complete the "Call-Out Culture" handout as they watch. Refer to the teacher version of the handout as you guide the class discussion.

Pressing play on the YouTube video will set third-party cookies controlled by Google if you are logged in to Chrome. See Google's cookie information for details.

3. Explain that at the end of the video, Aviva says, "The internet is supposed to be a tool, not a weapon." Ask: What does she mean by that, and how does it relate to conflict resolution and call-out culture?

4. Refer to Part 2 of the "Call-Out Culture" student handout and read aloud the quote by Loretta J. Ross to introduce the concept of "call-in culture." Then have students complete the reflection question and share out as time permits. 

Accidental Activist

Prep for teachers: 

  • Estimated activity time: 45 minutes
  • Preview the teacher version of the "Accidental Activist" handout for detailed facilitation guidance.
  • Make copies of the  "Accidental Activist" student handout  (or access the Google Docs version from the handout).

Activity Steps:

1. Say: "Calling out" people on social media who've done harmful things can be complicated. On one hand, it can be a way of holding someone accountable and stopping them from continuing the harm. But on the other hand, publicly shaming someone in this way often doesn't help them change or learn, and can also shut down the opportunity for open dialogue about the issue. Deciding whether "calling out" is the right or wrong thing to do may depend a lot on the actual circumstances.

If you haven't already taught the Quick Activity with your class, we recommend first showing the video  Does Canceling Someone Help Them Change?  so students can gain a deeper understanding of the concept of cancel culture. 

2. Distribute the "Accidental Activist" student handout and explain to students that they'll be using the "Feelings & Options" thinking routine to consider whether calling out someone or something online is an effective way to seek social change.

"Feelings & Options" is a thinking routine that supports communication, empathy, and thoughtful decision-making for digital dilemmas in social life. Learn more about teaching with dilemmas and thinking routines. 

3. After reading the dilemma as a class, have students work in pairs or groups to discuss and complete the questions that follow. Use the teacher version of the   "Accidental Activist" handout for detailed facilitation guidance and suggestions for an enriching discussion.

4. With students still in pairs or groups, have them discuss one or more of the scenarios in Part 2 of the handout. Then bring the class back together and have each group share out and discuss how their perspectives may have changed.

Call-In & Build Support

  • Preview and make copies of the project handout: "Call-In & Build Support"
  • Determine the mix of in- and out-of-class time students will need to spend on the assignment, and how students will share their work with the class and/or within their community.
  • Video: TED Talk – Don't Call People Out, Call Them In
  • Article: Why Scholar Loretta Ross Is 'Calling In' Callout Culture

1. Distribute the   "Call-In & Build Support" project handout and read through the directions as a class. Ensure that students have a good working understanding of "call-in" culture, including what it might look and sound like in practice. 

2. Allow students to brainstorm ideas for written or visual work before creating. 

3. Share student work with the class or in any forum you find appropriate.

Image courtesy of Allison Shelley/The Verbatim Agency for  American Education: Images of Teachers and Students in Action .

Daniel Vargas Campos

Daniel Vargas Campos is an Education Content Specialist at Common Sense Education. He develops research-backed educational resources that support young people to thrive in a digitally interconnected world. He has over 4 years of experience as a content creator in the education technology space. Prior to joining Common Sense Education, Daniel was a graduate instructor and researcher at the UC Berkeley Graduate School of Education where he studied the impact of educational technologies in the lives of students from non-dominant backgrounds. He holds an MA in Social and Cultural Studies from UC Berkeley and a BA in Interdisciplinary Studies from Tufts University.

Eisha Buch

As Director, Education Programs & Development for Common Sense Education, Eisha oversees education programming and content strategy for the Digital Citizenship Program. She has over 10 years of experience working in the K-12 education sector, starting out as a middle and high school teacher, and then focusing more deeply on curriculum development, teacher professional development and training, and program evaluation. Eisha develops research-based curricula to ensure the digital well-being of all students, with dedicated efforts to helping promote a positive learning culture around media and technology within schools. Eisha holds a B.A. in economics and political science from the University of Michigan and a M.A. in education from St. John's University. 

Related Content

Photo of a person gazing at a wall of televisions with the American flag

News Literacy Lessons to Help Students Understand Election Media

Use these free lesson plans to help students apply media literacy skills in a real-world context.

cancel culture research

Why Are Conspiracy Theories So Appealing?

Use this free lesson plan to help students think critically about conspiracy theories and how to respond to them.

Two students holding a video camera

Civics in Digital Life: Lesson Plans for Grades 8-12

Free lessons and activities to spark students' curiosity, critical thinking, and civic engagement.

Numbers, Facts and Trends Shaping Your World

Read our research on:

Full Topic List

Regions & Countries

  • Publications
  • Our Methods
  • Short Reads
  • Tools & Resources

Read Our Research On:

  • Americans and ‘Cancel Culture’: Where Some See Calls for Accountability, Others See Censorship, Punishment
  • Methodology

Table of Contents

  • Acknowledgments
  • Appendix: Coding information

The American Trends Panel survey methodology

The American Trends Panel (ATP), created by Pew Research Center, is a nationally representative panel of randomly selected U.S. adults. Panelists participate via self-administered web surveys. Panelists who do not have internet access at home are provided with a tablet and wireless internet connection. Interviews are conducted in both English and Spanish. The panel is being managed by Ipsos.

American Trends Panel recruitment surveys

Data in this report is drawn from the panel wave conducted Sept. 8 to 13, 2020. A total of 10,093 panelists responded out of 11,506 who were sampled, for a response rate of 88%. This does not include three panelists who were removed from the data due to extremely high rates of refusal or straightlining. The cumulative response rate accounting for nonresponse to the recruitment surveys and attrition is 5%. The break-off rate among panelists who logged on to the survey and completed at least one item is 1.7%. The margin of sampling error for the full sample of 10,093 respondents is plus or minus 1.6 percentage points.

This study featured a stratified random sample from the ATP. The sample was allocated according to the following strata, in order: tablet households, U.S.-born Hispanics, foreign-born Hispanics, high school education or less, foreign-born Asians, not registered to vote, people ages 18 to 34, uses internet weekly or less, non-Hispanic Black adults, nonvolunteers and all other categories not already falling into any of the above. Panelists who had not yet completed the annual profile survey were ineligible.

The ATP was created in 2014, with the first cohort of panelists invited to join the panel at the end of a large, national, landline and cellphone random-digit-dial survey that was conducted in both English and Spanish. Two additional recruitments were conducted using the same method in 2015 and 2017, respectively. Across these three surveys, a total of 19,718 adults were invited to join the ATP, of whom 9,942 (50%) agreed to participate.

In August 2018, the ATP switched from telephone to address-based recruitment. Invitations were sent to a random, address-based sample of households selected from the U.S. Postal Service’s Delivery Sequence File. Two additional recruitments were conducted using the same method in 2019 and 2020, respectively. Across these three address-based recruitments, a total of 17,161 adults were invited to join the ATP, of whom 15,134 (88%) agreed to join the panel and completed an initial profile survey. In each household, the adult with the next birthday was asked to go online to complete a survey, at the end of which they were invited to join the panel. Of the 25,076 individuals who have ever joined the ATP, 15,373 remained active panelists and continued to receive survey invitations at the time this survey was conducted.

The U.S. Postal Service’s Delivery Sequence File has been estimated to cover as much as 98% of the population, although some studies suggest that the coverage could be in the low 90% range. 1 The American Trends Panel never uses breakout routers or chains that direct respondents to additional surveys.

Weight dimensions

The ATP data was weighted in a multistep process that accounts for multiple stages of sampling and nonresponse that occur at different points in the survey process. First, each panelist begins with a base weight that reflects their probability of selection for their initial recruitment survey (and the probability of being invited to participate in the panel in cases where only a subsample of respondents were invited). The base weights for panelists recruited in different years are scaled to be proportionate to the effective sample size for all active panelists in their cohort. To correct for nonresponse to the initial recruitment surveys and gradual panel attrition, the base weights for all active panelists are calibrated to align with the population benchmarks identified in the accompanying table to create a full-panel weight.

For ATP waves in which only a subsample of panelists are invited to participate, a wave-specific base weight is created by adjusting the full-panel weights for subsampled panelists to account for any differential probabilities of selection for the particular panel wave. For waves in which all active panelists are invited to participate, the wave-specific base weight is identical to the full-panel weight.

In the final weighting step, the wave-specific base weights for panelists who completed the survey are again calibrated to match the population benchmarks specified above. These weights are trimmed (typically at about the 1st and 99th percentiles) to reduce the loss in precision stemming from variance in the weights. Sampling errors and test of statistical significance take into account the effect of weighting.

The following table shows the unweighted sample sizes and the error attributable to sampling that would be expected at the 95% level of confidence for different groups in the survey:

The unweighted sample sizes and the error attributable to sampling

Sample sizes and sampling errors for other subgroups are available upon request. In addition to sampling error, one should bear in mind that question wording and practical difficulties in conducting surveys can introduce error or bias into the findings of opinion polls.

How were the accountability and punishment open-ended responses coded?

Initial coding schemes for each question were derived from reading though the open-ended responses and identifying common themes. Using these themes, coders read each response and coded up to three themes for each response. (If a response was mentioned more than three themes, the first three mentioned were coded.)

After all responses were coded, similarities and groupings among codes both within and across the two questions about accountability and punishment became apparent. As such, these answers were grouped into broad areas that framed the biggest points of disagreement between these two groups.

We identified five key areas of disagreement in respondents’ arguments for why they held their views of calling out others for their posts on social media. The data breaks down across these main disputes as follows:

  • 25% of all adults address topics related to whether people who call out others are rushing to judge or are trying to be helpful
  • 14% center on whether calling out others on social media is a productive behavior
  • 10% focus on whether free speech or creating a comfortable environment online is more important
  • 8% address the differing agendas of those who call out others
  • 4% focus on whether speaking up is the best action to take if people find content offensive.

For the codes that make up each of these areas, see the Appendix .

© Pew Research Center, 2021

  • AAPOR Task Force on Address-based Sampling. 2016. “ AAPOR Report: Address-based Sampling .” ↩

Sign up for our weekly newsletter

Fresh data delivery Saturday mornings

Sign up for The Briefing

Weekly updates on the world of news & information

  • Free Speech & Press
  • Political Discourse

Americans’ views of offensive speech aren’t necessarily clear-cut

Many adults in east and southeast asia support free speech, are open to societal change, americans’ views of technology companies, most americans say a free press is highly important to society, ­most americans favor restrictions on false information, violent content online, most popular.

901 E St. NW, Suite 300 Washington, DC 20004 USA (+1) 202-419-4300 | Main (+1) 202-857-8562 | Fax (+1) 202-419-4372 |  Media Inquiries

Research Topics

  • Email Newsletters

ABOUT PEW RESEARCH CENTER  Pew Research Center is a nonpartisan fact tank that informs the public about the issues, attitudes and trends shaping the world. It conducts public opinion polling, demographic research, media content analysis and other empirical social science research. Pew Research Center does not take policy positions. It is a subsidiary of  The Pew Charitable Trusts .

© 2024 Pew Research Center

IMAGES

  1. 16 Cancel Culture Examples (2024)

    cancel culture research

  2. Awareness of ‘cancel culture’ has grown across U.S. demographic groups

    cancel culture research

  3. Cancel Culture defined

    cancel culture research

  4. How Cancel Culture Really Works: Being Critical in Media

    cancel culture research

  5. Awareness of ‘cancel culture’ has grown across U.S. demographic groups

    cancel culture research

  6. Awareness of ‘cancel culture’ has grown across U.S. demographic groups

    cancel culture research

COMMENTS

  1. Americans and 'Cancel Culture': Where Some ...

    And some argue that cancel culture doesn't even exist. To better understand how the U.S. public views the concept of cancel culture, Pew Research Center asked Americans in September 2020 to share - in their own words - what they think the term means and, more broadly, how they feel about the act of calling out others on social media.

  2. Awareness of 'cancel culture' has grown ...

    Americans have become increasingly aware of the phrase "cancel culture," according to a new Pew Research Center survey. And when it comes to calling out other people for posting potentially offensive content on social media, the public has become somewhat more likely to see this type of behavior as punishing people who didn't deserve it, rather than holding them accountable.

  3. Cancel Culture: the Phenomenon, Online Communities and Open Letters

    Cancel culture merupakan fenomena boikot massal yang dilakukan melalui media sosial. Penyebab dari cancel culture ini adalah kurangnya informasi yang valid, serta adanya mob mentality atau mental ...

  4. How Americans feel about 'cancel culture,' offensive speech

    Familiarity with the term cancel culture varied by age, gender and education level, but not political party affiliation, according to the same survey.. Younger adults were more likely to have heard about cancel culture than their older counterparts. Roughly two-thirds (64%) of adults under 30 said they'd heard a great deal or fair amount about cancel culture, compared with 46% of those ages ...

  5. Revisiting Cancel Culture

    In the hour-long video, she has identified seven "cancel culture tropes": a "presumption of guilt," "abstraction," "essentialism," "pseudo-moralism or pseudo-intellectualism," "no forgiveness," "the transitive property of cancellation," and "dualism.". This is where cancel culture can become dangerous.

  6. Cancel culture can be collectively validating for groups experiencing

    That is, an episode of cancel culture might provide the conditions for the harmed group to experience feelings of collective validation that, in turn, could increase feelings of empowerment that inspire or maintain collective action. To our knowledge, this research is the first social psychological analysis of the impact of cancel culture on ...

  7. CANCEL CULTURE: CANCELING by Samantha Haskell

    This research illuminates the process of cancel culture, specifically in how social media. trategies tha. determine if a cancellation istaking place. This provided the necessary d. ta to form a pilot model for canceling, w. ichdemonstrates the steps of a cancellation. T. y in determining the strategies Twitter us.

  8. #CancelCulture: Examining definitions and motivations

    While cancel culture has become a social media buzzword, scholarly understanding of this phenomenon is still at its nascent stage. To contribute to a more nuanced understanding of cancel culture, this study uses a sequential exploratory mixed-methods approach by starting with in-depth interviews with social media users (n = 20) followed by a national online survey (n = 786) in Singapore.

  9. Forming the Theoretical Framework of the "Cancel Culture": Conceptual

    Cancel culture and wokeism are dangerous trends that are increasing in their intensity and the self-destruction of Western civilization is a matter of time as the underlying historical myths and ...

  10. Opinion

    7. Cancel culture is most effective against people who are still rising in their fields, and it influences many people who don't actually get canceled. The point of cancellation is ultimately to ...

  11. Cancel Culture: A Critical Analysis

    About this book. "Cancel culture" has become one of the most charged concepts in contemporary culture and politics, but mainstream critiques from both the left and the right provide only snapshots of responses to the phenomenon. Takinga media and cultural studies perspective, this book traces the origins of cancel practices and discourses ...

  12. Cancel Culture: Myth or Reality?

    Abstract. In recent years, a progressive "cancel culture" in society, right-wing politicians and commentators claim, has silenced alternative perspectives, ostracized contrarians, and eviscerated robust intellectual debate, with college campuses at the vanguard of this development. These arguments can be dismissed as rhetorical dog whistles ...

  13. #Kancelkulture: An Analysis of Cancel Culture and Social Media Activism

    This study provides a definition of cancel culture through the perspectives of generation Z social media users and discusses the duality in which cancel culture is a form of social media activism, but also contributes to creating a spiral of silence online. Keywords: Cancel culture, STOPs Theory, Spiral of Silence, Social media, Twitter, Activism,

  14. Cancel Culture: Myth or Reality?

    In recent years, a progressive "cancel culture" in society, right-wing politicians and commentators claim, has silenced alternative perspectives, ostracized contrarians, and eviscerated robust intellectual debate, with college campuses at the vanguard of this development. These arguments can be dismissed as rhetorical dog whistles devoid of substantive meaning, myths designed to fire up ...

  15. #CancelCulture: Examining definitions and motivations

    Currently, scholars have expounded on cancel culture, social work practice and social service research. For example, Tandoc et al. (2022) found that cancel culture is more than just a mob engaged ...

  16. THE CURIOUS CASES OF CANCEL CULTURE

    cancel culture as a phenomenon. While intending to research and define what cancel culture is and understand the parameters of cancelations, I found the existence of a complex matrix through which users operate as digital trials and juries using social media platforms. To further understand cancel culture at both micro and macro levels, I

  17. PDF Cancel Culture

    8. 18 Actions taken to hold others accountable Censorship of speech or history Mean-spirited actions taken to cause others harm People cancelling anyone they disagree with Those who are challenged face consequences like being fired or boycotted An attack on traditional American society. way to call out racism, sexism, etc.

  18. The Mental Health Effects of Cancel Culture

    Positive Impact of Cancel Culture. Cancel culture can help combat wrongdoings and address inequalities. In 2016, for example, many members of the film community boycotted the Oscars because of the lack of diversity among nominees. This helped promote social change and, in 2019, the Oscars set a record for the most nominations for Black ...

  19. The second wave of "cancel culture"

    In his recent book Cancel This Book: The Progressive Case Against Cancel Culture, human rights lawyer and free speech advocate Dan Kovalik argues that cancel culture is basically a giant self-own ...

  20. Cancel culture

    Cancel culture is a cultural phenomenon in which an individual deemed to have acted or spoken in an unacceptable manner is ostracized, boycotted, ... A survey conducted in September 2020 on 10,000 Americans by Pew Research Center asked a series of different questions in regard to cancel culture, ...

  21. What Is Cancel Culture, and Does It Change Things for the Better?

    Grades: 8-12 Subjects: Digital Citizenship, SEL, Media Literacy, Social Studies What is cancel culture? A 2021 report from Pew Research Center found that the American public is deeply divided over how to define it. Some people view it as a way to hold a person or group of people accountable for a past misdeed. Others see it as an unfair judgment of someone's character, or even the practice of ...

  22. Cancel culture in a developing country: A belief in a just world

    Conversely, cancel culture has also been framed as a form of intolerance against opposing views. In this essay, I unpack the nuances and implications of cancel culture through Neil Alperstein's ...

  23. Cancel Culture: A Case Study on the Experiences of "Called Out" Person

    cancel culture experience loneliness, isolated and ostracization, which in some cases lead to depression, anxiety, and other associated condition (Spelmen, 2021). The negative impacts of cancel culture on the reputation of individuals in the online setting are the driving force of the researchers to conduct the study "Cancel Culture: A Case

  24. Methodology

    The American Trends Panel survey methodology. The American Trends Panel (ATP), created by Pew Research Center, is a nationally representative panel of randomly selected U.S. adults. Panelists participate via self-administered web surveys. Panelists who do not have internet access at home are provided with a tablet and wireless internet connection.